Bird Song

Biological Themes and Variations

Bird Song

Bird song is one of the most remarkable and impressive sounds in the natural world, and has inspired not only students of natural history, but also great writers, poets and composers. Extensively updated from the first edition, the main thrust of this book is to suggest that the two main functions of song are attracting a mate and defending territory. It shows how this evolutionary pressure has led to the amazing variety and complexity we see in the songs of different species throughout the world. Writing primarily for students and researchers in animal behavior, the authors review over 1000 scientific papers and reveal how scientists are beginning to unravel and understand how and why birds communicate with the elaborate vocalizations we call song. Highly illustrated throughout and written in straightforward language, Bird Song also holds appeal for amateur ornithologists with some knowledge of biology.


 Reviews:

"Bird Song is well referenced and clearly written; it provides good coverage of complex topics."
J. Burger, Choice

Adhikerana A. S . & Slater P. J. B . 1993. Singing interactions in coal tits, Parus ater: An experimental approach. Anim. Behav., 46, 1205–1211.
Adhikerana, A. S. 1992. The singing behaviour of coal tits (Parus ater). Ph.D. thesis, University of St Andrews.
Adret P . 1993. Operant conditioning, song learning and imprinting to taped song in the zebra finch. Anim. Behav., 46, 149–159.
Adret-Hausberger M . & Güttinger H. R . 1984. Constancy of basic pattern in the songs of two populations of starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). A comparison of song variation between sedentary and migratory populations. Z. Tierpsychol., 66, 309–327.
Adret-Hausberger M . & Jenkins P. F . 1988. Complex organisation of the warbling song in the European starling Sturnus vulgaris . Behaviour, 107, 138–156.
Adret-Hausberger M . 1983. Variations dialectales des sifflements de l'Etourneau sansonnet (Sturnus vulgaris) sedentaire en Bretagne. Z. Tierpsychol., 62, 55–71.
Adret-Hausberger M . 1989. The species-repertoire of whistled songs in the European starling: species-specific characteristics and variability. Bioacoustics, 2, 137–162.
Agate R. J ., Grisham W ., Wade J ., Mann S ., Wingfield J ., Schanen C ., Palotie A . & Arnold A. P . 2003. Neural, not gonadal, origin of brain sex differences in a gynandromorphic finch. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 100, 4873–4878.
Airey D. C . & DeVoogd T. J . 2000. Greater song complexity is associated with augmented song system anatomy in zebra finches. NeuroReport, 11, 2339–2344.
Airey D. C ., Buchanan K. L ., Catchpole C. K ., Szekely T . & DeVoogd T. J . 2000a. Song, sexual selection and a song control nucleus (HVC) in the brains of European sedge warblers. J. Neurobiol., 44, 1–6.
Airey D. C ., Castillo-Juarez H ., Casella G ., Pollak E. J . & DeVoogd T. J . 2000b. Variation in the volume of zebra finch song control nuclei is heritable: developmental and evolutionary implications. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B, 267, 2099–2104.
Alatalo R. V ., Lundberg A . & Glynn C . 1990. Singing rate and female attraction in the pied flycatcher: an experiment. Anim. Behav., 39, 601–603.
Alcock J . 1989. Animal Behaviour: An Evolutionary Approach. Sunderland, Mass.: Sinauer.
Alvarez-Borda B ., Haripal B . & Nottebohm F . 2004. Timing of brain-derived neurotrophic factor exposure affects life expectancy of new neurons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 101, 3957–3961.
Alvarez-Buylla A . & Nottebohm F . 1988. Migration of young neurons in adult avian brain. Nature, 335, 353–354.
Alvarez-Buylla A ., Kirn J. R . & Nottebohm F . 1990. Birth of projection neurons in adult avian brain may be related to perceptual or motor learning. Science, 249, 1444–1446.
Alvarez-Buylla A ., Theelen M . & Nottebohm F . 1988. Birth of projection neurons in the higher vocal center of the canary forebrain before, during and after song learning. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA, 85, 8722–8726.
Alvarez-Buylla B . & Nottebohm F . 2002. Gonads and singing play separate, additive roles in new neuron recruitment in adult canary brains. J. Neurosci., 22, 8684–8690.
Amrhein V . & Erne N . 2006. Dawn singing reflects past territorial challenges in the winter wren. Anim. Behav., 71, 1075–1080.
Amrhein V ., Kunc H. P . & Naguib M . 2004. Non-territorial nightingales prospect territories during the dawn chorus Proc . R. Soc. Lond. B (Suppl.), 271, S167–S169.
Anderson M. E . & Conner R. N . 1985. Cardinal song in three forest habitats in Eastern Texas. Wilson Bull., 97, 436–449.
Anderson R. C ., Searcy W. A . & Nowicki S . 2005. Partial song matching in an eastern population of song sparrows, Melospiza melodia . Anim. Behav., 69, 189–196.
Andersson M . 1994. Sexual Selection. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Andrew R. J . 1962. Evolution of intelligence and vocal mimicking. Science, 137, 585–589.
Arcese P ., Stoddard P. K . & Hiebert S. M . 1988. The form and function of song in female song sparrows. Condor, 90, 44–50.
Armstrong E. A . 1973. A Study of Bird Song. New York: Dover.
Arnold A. P . 1975. The effects of castration and androgen replacement on song, courtship and aggression in zebra finches Poephila guttata . J. Exp. Zool., 191, 309–326.
Arnold A. P . 1990. The passerine bird song system as a model in neuroendocrine research. J. Exp. Zool. Suppl., 4, 22–30.
Aubin T . & Jouventin P . 1998. Cocktail-party effect in king penguin colonies. Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B 265, 1665–1673.
Avery M . & Oring L. W . 1977. Song dialects in the bobolink (Dolichonyx oryzivorus). Condor, 79, 113–118.
Aylor D . 1972. Sound transmission through vegetation in relation to leaf area density, leaf width, and breadth of canopy. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 51, 411–414.
Bühner J . 1983. Songlearning in the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata):selectivity in the choice of a tutor and accuracy of song copies. Anim. Behav., 31, 231–7.
Badyaev A. V . & Leaf E. S . 1997. Habitat associations of song characteristics in Phylloscopus and Hippolais warblers. Auk, 114, 40–46.
Baker M. C . & Boylan J. T . 1995. A catalog of song syllables of indigo and lazuli buntings. Condor, 97, 1028–40.
Baker M. C . & Cunningham M. A . 1985. The biology of bird-song dialects. Behav. Brain Sci., 8, 85–133.
Baker M. C . & Mewaldt L. R . 1978. Song dialects as barriers to dispersal in white-crowned sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophrys nuttalli). Evolution, 32, 712–22.
Baker M. C . & Thompson D. B . 1985. Song dialects of white-crowned sparrows: historical processes inferred from patterns of geographic variation. Condor, 87, 127–41.
Baker M. C . 1974. Genetic structure of two populations of white-crowned sparrows with different song dialects. Condor, 76, 351–356.
Baker M. C . 1975. Song dialects and genetic differences in white-crowned sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophrys). Evolution, 29, 226–241.
Baker M. C . 1982. Vocal dialect recognition and population genetic consequences. Am. Zool., 22, 561–569.
Baker M. C . 1996. Depauperate meme pool of vocal signals in an island population of singing honeyeaters. Anim. Behav., 51, 853–858.
Baker M. C ., Baker E. M . & Baker M. S. A . 2001. Island and island-like effects on vocal repertoire of singing honeyeaters. Anim. Behav., 62, 767–774.
Baker M. C ., Bjerke T ., Lampe H . & Espmark Y . 1986. Sexual response of female great tits to variation in size of males song repertoire. Am. Nat., 128, 491–8.
Baker M. C ., Spitler-Nabors K. J . & Bradley D. C . 1981. Early experience determines song dialect responsiveness of female sparrows. Science, 214, 819–21.
Baker M. C ., Spitler-Nabors K. J . & Bradley D. C . 1982. The response of female mountain white-crowned sparrows to songs from their natal dialect and an alien dialect. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 10, 175–179.
Baker M. C ., Spitler-Nabors K. J ., Thompson A. D . & Cunningham M. A . 1987. Reproductive behaviour of female white-crowned sparrows: effect of dialects and synthetic hybrid songs. Anim. Behav., 35, 1766–1774.
Balaban E . 1988a. Cultural and genetic variation in swamp sparrows (Melospiza georgiana) I. Song variation, genetic variation, and their relationship. Behaviour, 105, 250–91.
Balaban E . 1988b. Cultural and genetic variation in swamp sparrows (Melospiza georgiana) II. Behavioural salience of geographic song variants. Behaviour, 105, 292–322.
Ball G. F . & MacDougall-Shackleton S. A . 2001. Sex differences in songbirds 25 years later: what have we learned and where do we go? Microsc. Res. Tech., 54, 327–34.
Ball G. F . 1999. The neuroendocrine basis of seasonal changes in vocal behavior among songbirds. In The Design of Animal Communication (ed. M. D. Hauser & M. Konishi ), pp. 213–53. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Ball G. F ., Auger C. J ., Bernard D. J . et al. 2004. Seasonal plasticity in the song control system. Multiple brain sites of steroid hormone action and the importance of the variation in song behavior. Ann. NY. Acad. Sci., 1016, 1–25.
Ball G. F ., Riters L. V . & Balthazart J . 2002. Neuroendocrinology of song behavior and avian brain plasticity: multiple sites of sex steroid hormones. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 23, 137–78.
Ballentine B ., Hyman J . & Nowicki S . 2004. Vocal performance influences female response to male bird song: an experimental test. Behav. Ecol., 15, 163–8.
Baptista L. F . & Catchpole C. K . 1989. Vocal mimicry and interspecific aggression in songbirds: Experiments using white-crowned sparrow imitation of song sparrow song. Behaviour, 109, 247–57.
Baptista L. F . & Morton M. L . 1981. Interspecific song acquisition by a white-crowned sparrow. Auk, 98, 383–5.
Baptista L. F . & Morton M. L . 1982. Song dialects and mate selection in montane white-crowned sparrows. Auk, 99, 537–47.
Baptista L. F . & Morton M. L . 1988. Song learning in montane white-crowned sparrows: from whom and when? Anim. Behav., 36, 1753–64.
Baptista L. F . & Petrinovich L . 1984. Social interaction, sensitive phases and the song template hypothesis in the white-crowned sparrow. Anim. Behav., 32, 172–81.
Baptista L. F . & Petrinovich L . 1986. Song development in the white-crowned sparrow: social factors and sex differences. Anim. Behav., 34, 1359–71.
Baptista L. F . & Schuchmann K.-L . 1990. Song learning in the Anna hummingbird (Calypte anna). Ethology, 84, 15–26.
Baptista L. F . & Trail P. W . 1992. The role of song in the evolution of passerine diversity. Syst. Biol., 41, 242–7.
Baptista L. F . 1975. Song dialects and demes in sedentary populations of the white-crowned sparrow (Zonotrichia leucophrys nuttalli). Publ. Zool. Univ. Calif., 105, 1–52.
Baptista L. F . 1977. Geographic variation in song and dialects of the Puget Sound white-crowned sparrow. Condor, 79, 356–70.
Baptista L. F . 1978. Territorial, courtship and duet songs of the Cuban grassquit (Tiaris canora). J. Ornithol., 119, 91–101.
Baptista L. F . 1988. Imitations of white-crowned sparrow songs by a song sparrow. Condor, 90, 486–9.
Baptista L. F . 1990. Dialectal variation in the raincall of the chaffinch (Fringilla coelebs). Die Vogelwarte, 35, 249–56.
Baptista L. F . 1996. Nature and its nurturing in avian vocal development. In: Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds (ed. D. E. Kroodsma & E. H. Miller ), pp. 39–60. Ithaca, NY, and London: Comstock Publishing Associates.
Baptista L. F ., Trail P. W ., DeWolfe B. B . & Morton M. L . 1993. Singing and its functions in female white-crowned sparrows. Anim. Behav., 46, 511–24.
Barrington D . 1773. Experiments and observations on the singing of birds. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., 63, 249–91.
Bartlett P . & Slater P. J. B . 1999. The effect of new recruits on the flock specific call of budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus). Ethol. Ecol. Evol., 11, 139–47.
Baylis J. R . 1982. Avian vocal mimicry: its function and evolution. In: Acoustic Communication in Birds (ed. D. E. Kroodsma & E. H. Miller ), pp. 51–83. New York: Academic Press.
Becker P. H . 1978. Der Einfluss des Lernens auf einfache und komplexe Gesangsstrophen der Sumpfmeise (Parus palustris). J. Orn., 119, 388–411.
Becker P. H . 1982. The coding of species-specific characteristics in bird sounds. In: Acoustic Communication in Birds, vol. 1 (ed. D. E. Kroodsma & E. H. Miller ), pp. 213–52. New York: Academic Press.
Beebee M. D . 2002. Song sharing by yellow warblers differs between two modes of singing: implications for song function. Condor, 104, 146–55.
Beebee M. D . 2004. The functions of multiple singing modes: experimental tests in yellow warblers, Dendroica petechia . Anim. Behav., 67, 1089–97.
Beecher M. D . & Brenowitz E. A . 2005. Functional aspects of song learning in songbirds. Trends Ecol. Evol., 20, 143–9.
Beecher M. D . & Campbell S. E . 2005. The role of unshared songs in singing interactions between neighbouring song sparrows. Anim. Behav., 70, 1297–304.
Beecher M. D . 1982. Development of parent-offspring recognition in birds. In: Experiential Factors in Perceptual Development. (ed. R. K. Aslin , J. R. Alberts & M. R. Petusa ), pp. 45–66. New York: Academic Press.
Beecher M. D . 1990. Evolution of parent-offspring recognition in swallows. In: Contemporary Issues in Comparative Psychology (ed. D. A. Dewsbury ), pp. 360–380. Sundenand, MA: Sinauer.
Beecher M. D ., Beecher I. M . & Hahn S . 1981. Parent-offspring recognition in bank swallows Riparia riparia: Development and acoustic basis. Anim. Behav., 29, 95–101.
Beecher M. D ., Campbell S. E . & Burt J. M . 1994a. Song perception in the song sparrow: birds classify by song type but not by singer. Anim. Behav., 47, 1343–51.
Beecher M. D ., Campbell S. E . & Nordby J. C . 2000b. Territory tenure in song sparrows is related to song sharing with neighbours, but not to repertoire size. Anim. Behav., 59, 29–37.
Beecher M. D ., Campbell S. E . & Stoddard P. K . 1994b. Correlation of song learning and territory establishment strategies in the song sparrow. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 91, 1450–54.
Beecher M. D ., Campbell S. E ., Burt J. M ., Hill C. E . & Nordby J. C . 2000a. Song-type matching between neighbouring song sparrows. Anim. Behav., 59, 21–7.
Beecher M. D ., Stoddard P. K ., Campbell S. E . & Horning C. L . 1996. Repertoire matching between neighbouring song sparrows. Anim. Behav., 51, 917–23.
Beer C. G . 1969. Laughing gull chicks: recognition of their parents voices. Science, 166, 1030–2.
Beletsky L. D . & Orians G. H . 1985. Nest-associated vocalizations of female red-winged blackbirds, Agelaius phoeniceus . Z. Tierpsychol., 69, 329–39.
Beletsky L. D . 1982. Vocal behavior of the Northern oriole. Wilson Bull., 94, 372–81.
Beletsky L. D . 1983a. Aggressive and pair-bond maintenance songs of female red-winged blackbirds (Agelaius phoeniceus) . Z. Tierpsychol., 62, 47–54.
Beletsky L. D . 1983b. Aggressive responses to ‘self’ songs by female red-winged blackbirds, Agelaius phoeniceus . Can. J. Zool., 61, 462–5.
Beletsky L. D . 1983c. Vocal mate recognition in male red-winged blackbirds, Agelaius phoeniceus . Behaviour, 84, 124–34.
Beletsky L. D . 1983d. An investigation of vocal recognition by voice in female red-winged blackbirds. Anim. Behav., 31, 355–62.
Beletsky L. D . 1985. Intersexual song answering in red-winged blackbirds. Can. J. Zool., 63, 735–7.
Beletsky L. D . 1989. Communication and the cadence of bird song. Am. Midl. Nat., 122, 298–306.
Bell B. D ., Borowiec M ., McConkey K. R . & Ranoszek E . 1997. Settlement, breeding success and song repertoires of monogamous and polygynous sedge warblers (Acrocephalus schoenobaenus). Die Vogelwarte 39, 87–94.
Bensch S . & Hasselquist D . 1992. Evidence for female choice in a polygynous warbler. Anim. Behav., 44, 301–11.
Berg K. S ., Brumfield R. T . & Apanius V . 2005. Phylogenetic and ecological determinants of the neotropical dawn chorus. Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B 273, 999–1005.
Bergman G . 1980. Die Veränderung der Gesangmelodie der Kohlmeise Parus major in Finnland und Schweden. Ornis Fenn., 57, 97–111.
Bergmann H.-H ., Flottmann E ., Heitkamp W ., Stehn-Nix K.-P . & Ubozak F . 1988. Die Osnabrücker Dialektkarte von Regenrufen des Buchfinken Fringilla coelebs im Jahre 1987. Vogelk. Ber. Nieders., 20, 89–96.
Bernard D. J ., Eens M . & Ball G. F . 1996. Age- and behavior-related variation in volumes of song control nuclei in male European starlings. J. Neurobiol., 30, 329–39.
Bertram B . 1970. The vocal behaviour of the Indian hill mynah. Anim. Behav. Monogr., 3, 81–192.
Bezzel E . 1988. Die Gesangzeiten desBuchfinken (Fringilla coelebs): eineRegionalstudie. J. Orn., 129, 71–81.
Birkhead T. R . & Møller A. P . 1992. Sperm Competition in Birds. London: Academic Press.
Birkhead T. R ., Buchanan K. L ., DeVoogd T. J ., et al. 1997. Song, sperm quality and testes asymmetry in the sedgewarbler. Anim. Behav., 53, 965–71.
Bitterbaum E . & Baptista L. F . 1979. Geographicalvariation in songs of California house finches (Carpodacusmexicanus). Auk, 96, 462–74.
Bjerke T. K . & Bjerke T. H . 1981. Songdialects in the redwing, Turdus iliacus . Ornis Scand., 12, 40–50.
Bjerke T. K . 1980. Continuityand change in dialect structure of redwing Turdusiliacus . Fauna norv. Ser. C Cinclus, 3, 73–9.
Bjorklund M . 1989. Microgeographicvariation in the song of the scarlet rosefinch Carpodacuserythrinus . Ornis Scand., 20, 255–64.
Bjorklund M ., Westman B . & Allander K . 1989. Song in Swedish great tits: intra- or intersexualcommunication. Behaviour, 111, 257–69.
Bolhuis J. J . & Eda-Fujiwara H . 2003. Bird brains and songs:neural mechanisms of birdsong perception and memory. Anim. Biol., 53, 129–45.
Bolhuis J. J . & Gahr M . 2006. Neural mechanisms ofbirdsong memory. Nature Reviews Neurosci., 7, 347–57.
Borror D. J . 1981. The songsand singing behavior of the red-eyed vireo. Condor, 83, 217–28.
Borror D. J . 1987. Song inthe white-eyed vireo. Wilson Bull., 99, 377–97.
Boseret G ., Carere C ., Ball G. F . & Balthazart J . 2005. Socialcontext affects testosterone-induced singing and the volume of song controlnuclei in male canaries Serinus canaria . J. Neurobiol., 66, 1044–60.
Bottjer S. W ., Miesner E. A . & Arnold A. P . 1984. Forebrain lesionsdisrupt development but not maintenance of song in passerine birds. Science, 224, 901–3.
Boughey M. J . & Thompson N. S . 1981. Songvariety in the Brown Thrasher (Toxostoma rufum). Z. Tierpsychol., 56, 47–58.
Brémond J.-C . 1968. Recherches sur lasemantique et les elements vecteurs d'information dans les signaux acoustiquesdu rouge-gorge Erithacus rubecula . La Terre et la Vie, 2, 109–220.
Brémond J.-C . 1976. Specific recognition in the songof Bonelli's warbler Phylloscopus bonelli . Behaviour, 58, 99–116.
Brémond J.-C . 1978. Acousticcompetition between the songs of the wren (Troglodytestroglodytes) and the songs of other species. Behaviour, 65, 89–98.
Brackenbury J. H . 1980. Respirationand production of sounds by birds. Biol. Rev., 55, 363–78.
Brackenbury J. H . 1982. The structural basis of voice production and its relationship to sound characteristics. In: Acoustic Communication in Birds, vol. 1 (ed. D. E. Kroodsma & E. H. Miller ), pp. 214–52. New York: Academic Press.
Brackenbury J. H . 1989. Functions of the syrinx and the control of sound production. In: Form and Function in Birds, (ed. A. S. King & J. McLelland ), pp. 193–220. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Bradley R. A . 1981. Songvariation in a population of white-eyed vireos (Vireogriseus). Auk, 98, 80–7.
Brainard M. S . & Doupe A. J . 2000. Auditoryfeedback in learning and maintenance of vocal behaviour. NatureReviews, 1, 31–40.
Brainard M. S . & Doupe A. J . 2002. What songbirds teach usabout learning. Nature, 417, 351–358.
Breitwisch R . & Whitesides G. H . 1987. Directionalityof singing and non-singing behaviour of mated and unmated northernmockingbirds, Mimus polyglottos . Anim. Behav., 35, 331–9.
Brenowitz E. A . & Arnold A. P . 1986. Interspecificcomparisons of the size of neural song control regions and song complexity in duetting birds: evolutionary implications. J. Neurosci., 6, 2875–9.
Brenowitz E. A . 1982a. Theactive space of red-winged blackbird song. J. comp. Physiol., 147, 511–22.
Brenowitz E. A . 1982b. Aggressiveresponse of red-winged blackbirds to mockingbird song imitation. Auk, 99, 584–6.
Brenowitz E. A . 1991a. Evolutionof the vocal control system in the avian brain. Seminarsin the Neurosciences, 3, 399–407.
Brenowitz E. A . 1991b. Alteredperception of species-specific song by birds after lesions of a forebrainnucleus. Science, 251, 303–5.
Brenowitz E. A ., Nalls B ., Wingfield J. C . & Kroodsma D. E . 1991. Seasonal changes in avian song nuclei without seasonalchanges in song repertoire. J. Neurosci., 11, 1367–74.
Brindley E. L . 1991. Responseof European robins to playback of song: neighbour recognition and overlapping. Anim. Behav., 41, 503–12.
Brockway B. F . 1965. Stimulationof ovarian development and egg-laying by male courtship vocalizations inbudgerigars Melopsittacus undulatus . Anim. Behav., 13, 575–9.
Brooke M.L . 1978. Sexual differences inthe voice and individual vocal recognition in the manx shearwater Puffinuspuffinus . Anim. Behav., 26, 622–9.
Brooks R. J . & Falls J. B . 1975a. Individual recognitionby song in white-throated sparrows. I. Can. J. Zool., 53, 879–88.
Brooks R. J . & Falls J. B . 1975b. Individual recognitionby song in white-throated sparrows. III. Can. J. Zool., 53, 1749–61.
Brown C. H . 1982. Ventroloquialand locatable vocalizations in birds. Z. Tierpsychol., 59, 338–50.
Brown E. D . & Farabaugh S. M . 1991. Songsharing in a group-living songbird, the Australian magpie, Gymnorhinatibicen. Part III. Sex specificity and individual specificity of vocalparts in communal chorus and duet songs. Behaviour, 118, 244–74.
Brown E. D ., Farabaugh S. M . & Veltman C. J . 1988. Song sharing in a group-livingsongbird, the Australian magpie, Gymnorhinatibicen Part I. Vocal sharing within and among social groups. Behaviour, 104, 1–28.
Brown R. N . & Lemon R. E . 1979. Structure and evolutionof song form in the wrens Thryothorus sinaloaand T. felix . Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 5, 111–31.
Brown T. J . & Handford P . 2003. Why birds sing at dawn:the role of consistent sound transmission. Ibis, 145, 120–9.
Brumm H . & Slabbekoorn H . 2005. Acoustic communication in noise. Adv. Study Behav., 35, 151–209.
Brumm H . & Slater P. J. B . 2006a. Ambient noise, motor fatigue andserial redundancy in chaffinch song. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 60, 475–81.
Brumm H . & Slater P. J. B . 2006b. Animalscan vary signal amplitude with receiver distance: evidence from zebra finch song. Anim. Behav., 71, 699–705.
Brumm H . & Todt D . 2002. Noise-dependent song amplituderegulation in a territorial songbird. Anim. Behav., 63, 891–7.
Brumm H . & Todt D . 2003. Facing the rival: directionalsinging behaviour in nightingales. Behaviour, 140, 43–53.
Brumm H . 2004. The impact ofenvironmental noise on song amplitude in a terrirorial bird. J. Anim. Ecol., 73, 434–40.
Brumm H . 2006. Signalling throughacoustic windows: nightingales avoid interspecific competition by short-termadjustment of song timing. J. comp. Physiol. A, 192, 1279–85.
Buchanan K. L . & Catchpole C K . 2000a. Extra-pair paternityin the socially monogamous sedge warbler (Acrocephalusschoenobaenus) as revealed by multilocus DNA fingerprinting. Ibis, 142, 12–20.
Buchanan K. L . & Catchpole C. K . 1997. Femalechoice in the sedge warbler, Acrocephalusschoenobaenus: multiple cues from song and territory quality. Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B 264, 521–6.
Buchanan K. L . & Catchpole C. K . 2000b. Song asan indicator of male parental effort in the sedge warbler. Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B 267, 321–6.
Buchanan K. L ., Catchpole C. K ., Lewis J. W . & Lodge A . 1999. Song as an indicator of parasitismin the sedge warbler. Anim. Behav., 57, 307–14.
Buchanan K. L ., Leitner S ., Spencer K. A ., Goldsmith A. R . & Catchpole C. K . 2004. Developmental stress selectivelyaffects the song control nucleus HVC in the zebra finch. Proc. R. Soc. Lond, B 271, 2381–2386.
Buchanan K. L ., Spencer K. A ., Goldsmith A. R . & Catchpole C. K . 2003. Song as an honest signal of pastdevelopmental stress in the European starling (Sturnusvulgaris). Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B 270, 1149–56.
Burd G. D . & Nottebohm F . 1985. Ultrastructural characterizationof synaptic terminals formed on newly generated neurons in a song controlnucleus of the adult canary forebrain. J. comp. Neurol., 240, 143–52.
Burnell K . 1998. Cultural variation insavannah sparrow, Passerculus sandwichensis, songs: an analysis using the meme concept. Anim. Behav., 56, 995–1003.
Burt J. M . & Vehrencamp S. L . 2005. Dawn chorus as an interactive communication network. In: Animal Communication Networks (ed by P McGregor ), pp. 320–43. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Burt J. M ., Bard S. C ., Campbell S. E . & Beecher M. D . 2002a. Alternative forms of song matching in song sparrows. Anim. Behav., 63, 1143–51.
Burt J. M ., Campbell S. E . & Beecher M. D . 2002b. Song type matching as threat: atest using interactive playback. Anim. Behav., 62, 1163–70.
Burt J. M ., Lent K. L ., Beecher M. D . & Brenowitz E.A . 2000. Lesions of the anterior forebrain song control pathway infemale canaries affect song perception in an operant task. J. Neurobiol., 42, 1–13.
Byers B. E . 1995. Songtypes, repertoires and song variability in a population of chestnut-sidedwarblers. Condor, 97, 390–401.
Byers B. E . 1996a. Geographicvariation of song form within and among chestnut-sided warbler populations. Auk, 113, 288–99.
Byers B. E . 1996b. Messagesencoded in the songs of chestnut-sided warblers. Anim. Behav., 52, 691–705.
Calder W.A . 1970. Respiration during songin the canary (Serinus canaria). Comp. Biochem . Physiol., 32, 251–8.
Canady R. A ., Kroodsma D. E . & Nottebohm F . 1984. Population differencesin complexity of a learned skill are correlated with the brain space involved. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA, 81, 6232–4.
Capp M. S . & Searcy W. A . 1991. An experimental studyof song type function in the bobolink (Dolichonyxoryzivorus). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 28, 179–86.
Capp M. S . 1992. Tests ofthe function of the song repertoire in bobolinks. Condor, 94, 468–79.
Catchpole C. K . & Baptista L. F . 1988. Atest of the competition hypothesis of vocal mimicry, using song sparrowimitation of white-crowned sparrow song. Behaviour, 106, 119–28.
Catchpole C. K . & Komdeur J . 1993. The song of theSeychelles warbler Acrocephalus sechellensis, an island endemic. Ibis, 135, 190–5.
Catchpole C. K . & Leisler B . 1986. Interspecificterritorialism in reed warblers: a local effect revealed by playback experiments. Anim. Behav., 34, 299–330.
Catchpole C. K . & Leisler B . 1989. Variation in the songof the aquatic warbler Acrocephalus paludicolain response to playback of different song structures. Behaviour, 108, 125–38.
Catchpole C. K . & McGregor P. K . 1985. Sexualselection, song complexity and plumage dimorphism in European buntings of thegenus Emberiza Anim. Behav., 33, 1378–80.
Catchpole C. K . & Rowell A . 1993. Song sharing and localdialects in a population of the European wren Troglodytestroglodytes . Behaviour, 125, 67–78.
Catchpole C. K . 1973. Thefunctions of advertising song in the sedge warbler Acrocephalusschoenobaenus and the reed warbler A. scirpaceus. Behaviour, 46, 300–20.
Catchpole C. K . 1976. Temporaland sequential organisation of song in the sedge warbler (Acrocephalusschoenobaenus). Behaviour, 59, 226–46.
Catchpole C. K . 1978. Interspecificterritorialism and competition in Acrocephaluswarblers as revealed by playback experiments in areas of sympatry and allopatry. Anim. Behav., 26, 1072–80.
Catchpole C. K . 1979. Vocal Communicationin Birds. London: Edward Arnold.
Catchpole C. K . 1980. Sexualselection and the evolution of complex songs among warblers of the genus Acrocephalus . Behaviour, 74, 149–66.
Catchpole C. K . 1982. The evolution of bird sounds in relation to mating and spacing behavior. In: Acoustic Communication in Birds (ed. D. Kroodsma ' E. H. Miller ), pp. 297–319. New York: Academic Press.
Catchpole C. K . 1983. Variationin the song of the great reed warbler Acrocephalusarundinaceus in relation to mate attraction and territorial defence. Anim. Behav., 31, 1217–25.
Catchpole C. K . 1986. Songrepertoires and reproductive success in the great reed warbler Acrocephalusarundinaceus . Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 19, 439–45.
Catchpole C. K . 1987. Bird song, sexual selection and female choice. TrendsEcol. Evol., 2, 94–7.
Catchpole C. K . 1989. Pseudoreplicatonand external validity: some thoughts on the suggested redesign of playbackexperiments in avian bioacoustics. TrendsEcol. Evol., 4, 286–7.
Catchpole C. K . 1992. Integrating playback: a wider context. In: Playback and Studies of Animal Communication (ed. P. K. McGregor ), pp. 35–46. New York: Plenum Press.
Catchpole C. K . 2000. Sexualselection and the evolution of song and brain structure in Acrocephaluswarblers. Adv. Study Behav., 29, 45–97.
Catchpole C. K ., Dittami J . & Leisler B . 1984. Differential responses to malesong repertoires in female songbirds implanted with oestradiol. Nature, 312, 563–4.
Catchpole C. K ., Leisler B . & Dittami J . 1986. Sexual differences in theresponses of captive great reed warblers Acrocephalusarundinaceus, to variation in song structure and size. Ethology, 73, 69–77.
Chappuis C . 1971. Un example del'influence du milieu sur les émissions vocales des oiseaux: L'évolution deschants en forêt équatoriale. Terre et Vie, 25, 183–202.
Chatfield C . ' Lemon R. E . 1970. Analysing sequences of behaviouralevents. J. theor. Biol, 29, 427–45.
Chilton G . & Lein M. R . 1996. Long-termchanges in songs and song dialect boundaries of Puget sound white-crownedsparrows. Condor, 98, 567–80.
Chilton G ., Lein M. R . & Baptista L. F . 1990. Matechoice by female white-crowned sparrows in a mixed dialect population. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 27, 223–7.
Chilton G ., Wiebe M. O . & Handford P . 2002. Large scale geographic variation in songs of Gambel'swhite-crowned sparrows. Condor, 104, 378–86.
Clark A. J ., Boersma P. D . & Olmsted D. M . 2006. Name that tune: calldiscrimination and individual recognition in Magellanic penguins. Anim. Behav., 72, 1141–8.
Clayton N. S . 1987. Song tutorchoice in zebra finches. Anim. Behav., 35, 714–21.
Clayton N. S . 1988. Songdiscrimination learning in zebra finches. Anim. Behav., 36, 1016–24.
Clayton N. S . 1990a. Subspeciesrecognition and song learning in zebra finches. Anim. Behav., 40, 1009–17.
Clayton N. S . 1990b. Assortativemating in zebra finch subspecies, Taeniopygiaguttata guttata and T. g. castanotis . Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B 330, 351–70.
Cody M. L . & Brown J. H . 1969. Songasynchrony in neighbouring bird species. Nature, 222, 778–80.
Coles R. B ., Lewis D. B ., Hill K. G ., Hutchings M. E . & Gower D. M . 1980. Directional hearing in the Japanese quail (Coturnixcoturnix japonica). II. Cochlear physiology. J. exp. Biol., 86, 153–70.
Collins S . 2004. Vocal fighting and flirting: the functions of birdsong. In: Nature's Music: The Science of Birdsong (ed. P. Marler ' H. Slabbekoorn ), pp. 42–82. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Cooney R . & Cockburn A . 1995. Territorial defence isthe major function of female song in the superb fairy-wren, Maluruscyaneus . Anim. Behav. 49, 1635–47.
Cooper B. G . & Goller F . 2004. Multimodal signals:enhancement and constraint of song motor patterns by visual display. Science, 303, 544–6.
Cortopassi K. A . & Bradbury J. W . 2000. Thecomparison of harmonically rich sounds using spectrographic cross-correlationand principal coordinates analysis. Bioacoustics, 111, 89–127.
Cosens S. E . & Falls J. B . 1984a. A comparison of soundpropagation and song frequency in temperate marsh and grassland habitats. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 15, 161–70.
Cosens S. E . & Falls J. B . 1984b. Structure and use ofsong in the yellow-headed blackbird (Xanthocephalusxanthocephalus). Z. Tierpsychol., 66, 227–41.
Craig W . 1943. The song of the woodpewee Myiochanes virens Linnaeus: Astudy of bird music. N.Y. State Mus. Bull., 334, 1–186.
Cresswell W . 1994. Song as a pursuitdeterrent signal, and its occurrence relative to other anti-predator behavioursof skylarks (Alauda arvensis) on attackby merlins (Falco columbarius). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 34, 217–23.
Cunningham M. A . & Baker M. C . 1983. Vocal learning inwhite-crowned sparrows: sensitive phase and song dialects. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 13, 259–69.
Cuthill I. C . & Hindmarsh A . 1985. Increase in starlingsong activity with removal of mate. Anim. Behav., 33, 326–8.
Cuthill I. C . & Macdonald W. A . 1990. Experimentalmanipulation of the dawn and dusk chorus in the blackbird Turdusmerula . Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 26, 209–16.
Cynx J ., Lewis R ., Tavel B . ', Tse H . 1998. Amplituderegulation of vocalizations in noise by a songbird, Taeniopygiaguttata . Anim. Behav., 56, 107–13.
Cynx J ., Williams H . & Nottebohm F . 1992. Hemispheric differences in avian song discrimination. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 89, 1372–75.
Dabelsteen T . & McGregor P. K . 1996. Dynamic acoustic communication and interactive playback. In: Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds (ed. D. E. Kroodsma ' E. H. Miller ), pp. 398–408. Ithaca, NY, and London: Comstock Publishing Associates.
Dabelsteen T . & Pedersen S. B . 1991. A portable digital sound emitterfor interactive playback of animal vocalisations. Bioacoustics, 3, 193–206.
Dabelsteen T . & Pedersen S. B . 1993. Song-based species discriminationand behaviour assessment by female blackbirds, Turdusmerula . Anim. Behav., 45, 759–71.
Dabelsteen T . & Pederson S . 1990. Song and aggressiveresponses of blackbirds, Turdus merula:evidence from interactive playback experiments with territory owners. Anim. Behav. 40, 1158–68.
Dabelsteen T . 1992. Interactive playback: a finely tuned response. In: Playback and Studies of Animal Communication (ed. P. K. McGregor ), pp. 97–110. New York: Plenum Press.
Dabelsteen T ., Larsen O. N . & Pedersen S. B . 1993. Habitat-induced degradation ofsound signals: quantifying the effects of communication sounds and birdlocation of blur ratio, excess attenuation, and signal-to-noise ratio inblackbird song. J. Acoust. Soc. Amer., 93, 2206–20.
Dale S ., Amundsen T ., Lifjelt J. T . & Slagsvold T . 1990. Matesampling behaviour of female pied flycatchers: evidence for active mate choice. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 27, 87–91.
Dale S ., Rinden H . & Slagsvold T . 1992. Competition for a mate restricts mate search of female piedflycatchers. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 30, 165–76.
Darwin C . 1871. The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex. London: John Murray.
Date E. M . & Lemon R. E . 1993. Soundtransmission: a basis for dialects in birdsong? Behaviour, 124, 291–312.
Dave A . & Margoliash D . 2000. Song replay during sleep and computational rules for sensorimotor vocal learning. Science, 290, 812–16.
Davies N. B ., Kilner R. M . & Noble D. G . 1998. Nestling cuckoos Cuculuscanorus exploit hosts with begging calls that mimic a brood. Proc. R. Soc. Lond, B 265, 673–8.
Dawkins M. S . & Guilford T . 1991. The corruption ofhonest signalling. Anim. Behav., 41, 865–74.
Dawson S. M . & Jenkins P. F . 1983. Chaffinch songrepertoires and the Beau Geste hypothesis. Behaviour, 87, 256–69.
de Kort S. R . & ten Cate C . 2001. Response tointerspecific vocalizations is affected by degree of relatedness in Streptopeliadoves. Anim. Behav., 61, 239–247.
Deecke V. B ., Ford J. K. B . & Spong P . 1999. Quantifying complex patterns ofbioacoustic variation: use of a neural network to compare killer whale (Orcinusorca) dialects. J. Acoust. Soc. Amer., 105, 2499–507.
delNegro C ., Gahr M ., Leboucher G . & Kreutzer M . 1998. The selectivity of sexualresponses to song displays: effects of partial chemical lesion of HVC in femalecanaries. Behav. Brain Res., 96, 151–9.
delNegro C ., Kreutzer M . & Gahr M . 2000. Sexual stimulatingsignals of canary (Serinus canaria)songs: evidence for a female-specific auditory representation in the HVCnucleus during the breeding season. Behav. Neurosci., 114, 526–42.
Depraz V ., Leboucher G . & Kreutzer M . 2000. Early tutoring and adult reproductive behaviour in femaledomestic canary (Serinus canaria). Anim. Cogn., 3, 45–51.
Derégnaucourt S ., Mitra P. P ., Fehér O ., Pytte C. ' Tchernichovski O . 2005. Howsleep affects the developmental learning of bird song. Nature, 433, 710–16.
DeVoogd T. J ., Krebs J. R ., Healy S. D . & Purvis A . 1993. Relations between song repertoire size and the volume ofbrain nuclei related to song: comparative evolutionary analyses amongst oscinebirds. Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B 254, 75–82.
DeVoogd., T. J . 1991. Endocrinemodulation of the development and adult function of the avian song system. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 16, 41–66.
DeWolfe B. B ., Baptista L. F . & Petrinovich L . 1989. Song development andterritory establishment in Nuttall's white-crowned sparrows. Condor, 91, 397–407.
DeWolfe B. B ., Kaska D. D . & Peyton L. J . 1974. Prominent variations in the songsof Gambel's white-crowned sparrows. Bird Banding, 45, 224–52.
Dilger W. C . 1953. Duettingin the crimson-breasted barbet. Condor, 55, 220–1.
Dobkin D. S . 1979. Functionaland evolutionary relationships of vocal copying phenomena in birds. Z. Tierpsychol., 50, 348–63.
Dobson C. W . & Lemon R. E . 1975. Re-examination of themonotony-threshold hypothesis in bird song. Nature, 257, 126–8.
Dobson C. W . & Lemon R. E . 1979. Markov sequences insongs of American thrushes. Behaviour, 68, 86–105.
Dooling R . 2004. Audition: can birds hear everything they sing? In Nature's Music: The Science of Birdsong (ed. P. Marler ' H. Slabbekoorn ), pp. 209–28. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Doupe A. J . & Konishi M . 1991. Song-selective auditorycircuits in the vocal control system of the zebra finch. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA, 88, 11339–43.
Doupe A. J . & Kuhl P. K . 1999. Birdsong and humanspeech: Common themes and mechanisms. Annu. Rev. Neurosci., 22, 567–631.
Doutrelant C . & Lambrechts M. M . 2001. Macrogeographicvariation in song a test of competition and habitat effects in blue tits. Ethology, 107, 533–44.
Doutrelant C ., Aubin T ., Hitier S . & Lambrechts M. M . 1998. Twodistinct song populations of blue tit Paruscaeruleus in the French Mediterranean. Bioacoustics, 9, 1–16.
Dowsett-Lemaire F . 1979. Theimitative range of the song of the marsh warbler Acrocephaluspalustris, with special reference to imitations of African birds. Ibis, 121, 453–68.
Draganoiu T. I ., Nagle L ., Musseau R . & Kreutzer M . 2006. In a songbird, the black redstart, parents use acousticcues to discriminate between their different fledglings. Anim. Behav., 71, 1039–46.
Duffy D. L . & Ball G. F . 2002. Song predictsimmunocompetence in male European starlings (Sturnusvulgaris). Proc. R. Soc. Lond, B 269, 847–52.
Dufty A. M . 1985. Songsharing in the brown-headed cowbird (Molothrusater). Z. Tierpsychol., 69, 177–190.
Dussourd N . & Ritchison G . 2003. Singing behavior of maleyellow-breasted chats: repertoires, rates, reproductive success, and acomparison with other wood-warblers. WilsonBull., 115, 52–7.
Eales L. A . 1985. Songlearning in zebra finches: Some effects of song model availability on what islearnt and when. Anim. Behav., 33, 1293–300.
Eales L. A . 1989. Theinfluences of visual and vocal interaction on song learning in zebra finches. Anim. Behav., 37, 507–8.
Eberhardt L. S . 1994. Oxygen consumption during singing by male Carolina wrens (Thryothorusludovicianus). Auk, 111, 124–30.
Eens M . 1997. Understandingthe complex song of the European starling: an integrated ethological approach. Adv. Study Behav., 26, 355–434.
Eens M ., Pinxten R . & Verheyen R. F . 1989. Temporal and sequential organisation of song bouts in the starling. Ardea, 77, 75–86.
Eens M ., Pinxten R . & Verheyen R. F . 1990. On the function of singing andwing-waving in the European starling Sturnusvulgaris L. Bird Study, 37, 48–52.
Eens M ., Pinxten R . & Verheyen R. F . 1991a. Male song as a cue for mate choice in the European starling. Behaviour, 116, 210–38.
Eens M ., Pinxten R . & Verheyen R. F . 1991b. Organisation of song in the European starling: species specificity and individual differences. Belg. J. Zool., 121, 257–78.
Eens M ., Pinxten R . & Verheyen R. F . 1992. Song learning in captive European starlings, Sturnus vulgaris . Anim. Behav., 44, 1131–43.
Eens M ., Pinxten R . & Verheyen R. F . 1993. Function of the song and song repertoire in the European starling (Sturnus vulgaris): an aviary experiment. Behaviour, 125, 51–66.
Ellinger N . & Hödl W . 2003. Habitat acoustics of a neotropical lowland rainforest. Bioacoustics, 13, 297–321.
Emlen S. T ., Rising J. D . & Thompson W. L . 1975. A behavioural and morphological study of sympatry in the indigo and lazuli buntings of the great plains. Wilson Bull., 87, 145–302.
Emlen S.T . 1972. An experimental analysis of the parameters of bird song eliciting species recognition. Behaviour, 41, 130–71.
Eriksson D . & Wallin L . 1986. Male bird song attracts females - a field experiment. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 19, 297–9.
Espmark Y . 1982. Microgeographical and individual variation in the morphology of song of the redwing Turdus iliacus . Fauna norv. ser. C Cinclus, 5, 73–83.
Espmark Y . 1995. Individual and local variations in the song of the snow bunting Plectrophenax nivalis on Spitzbergen. Bioacoustics, 6, 117–34.
Espmark Y. O . & Lampe H. M . 1993. Variations in the song of the pied flycatcher within and between breeding seasons. Bioacoustics, 5, 33–65.
Espmark Y. O ., Lampe H. M . & Bjerke T. K . 1989. Song conformity and continuity in song dialects of redwings Turdus iliacus and some ecological correlates. Ornis Scand., 20, 1–12.
Evans E. J . & Bazley E. N . 1956. The absorption of sound in air at audio frequencies. Acustica, 6, 238–45.
Falls J. B . & Brooks R. J . 1975. Individual recognition of song in white-throated sparrows. II. Can. J. Zool., 53, 1412–20.
Falls J. B . & Krebs J. R . 1975. Sequence of songs in repertoires of western meadowlarks (Sturnella neglecta). Can. J. Zool., 53, 1165–78.
Falls J. B . 1978. Bird song and territorial behavior. In: Advances in the Study of Communication and Affect, vol. 4: Aggression, Dominance and Individual Spacing (ed. L. Krames , P. Pliner & T. Alloway ), pp. 61–89. New York: Plenum Press.
Falls J. B . 1985. Song matching in Western Meadowlarks. Can. J. Zool., 63, 2520–4.
Falls J. B . 1987. Does song deter intrusion in white-throated sparrows Zonotrichia alibicollis? Can. J. Zool., 66, 206–11.
Falls J. B . 1992. Playback: a historical perspective. In: Playback and Studies of Animal Communication, (ed. P. K. McGregor ), pp. 11–33. New York: Plenum.
Falls J. B ., Horn A. G . & Dickinson T. E . 1988. How western meadowlarks classify their song: evidence from song matching. Anim. Behav., 36, 579–85.
Farabaugh S. M . 1982. The ecological and social significance of duetting. In: Acoustic Communication in Birds (ed. D. E. Kroodsma & E. H. Miller ), pp. 85–124. New York: Academic Press.
Farabaugh S. M ., Brown E. D . & Veltman C. J . 1988. Song sharing in a group-living songbird, the Australian magpie. Part II. Vocal sharing between territorial neighbors, within and between geographic regions, and between sexes. Behaviour, 104, 105–25.
Farabaugh S. M ., Linzenbold A . & Dooling R. J . 1994. Vocal plasticity in budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus): evidence for social factors in the learning of contact calls. J. comp. Psychol., 108, 81–92.
Felsenstein J . 2004. Inferring Phylogenies. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.
Ficken R. W ., Ficken M. S . & Hailman J. P . 1974. Temporal pattern shifts to avoid acoustic interference in singing birds. Science, 183, 762–3.
Ficken R. W ., Popp J. W . & Matthiae P. E . 1985. Avoidance of acoustic interference by ovenbirds. Wilson Bull., 97, 569–71.
Fisher R. A . 1954. Evolution and bird sociality. In: Evolution as a Process (ed. J. Huxley , A. Hardy & E. Ford ), pp. 71–83. London: Allen & Unwin.
Forstmeier W . & Balsby J.S . 2002. Why mated dusky warblers sing so much: territory guarding and male quality announcement. Behaviour, 139, 89–111.
Forstmeier W . & Leisler B . 2004. Repertoire size, sexual selection and offspring viability in the great reed warbler: changing patterns in space and time. Behav. Ecol., 15, 555–63.
Forstmeier W ., Hasselquist D ., Bensch S . & Leisler B . 2006. Does song reflect age and viability? A comparison between two populations of the great reed warbler Acrocephalus arundinaceus . Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 59, 634–43.
Fotheringham J. R . & Ratcliffe L . 1995. Song degradation and estimation of acoustic distance in black-capped chickadees (Parus atricapillus). Can. J. Zool., 73, 858–68.
Fotheringham J. R ., Martin P. R . & Ratcliffe L . 1997. Song transmission and auditory perception of distance in wood warblers (Parulinae). Anim. Behav., 53, 1271–85.
Franz M . & Goller F . 2002. Respiratory units of motor production and song imitation in the zebra finch. J. Neurobiol., 51, 129–41.
Gaddis P. K . 1983. Differential usage of song types by plain, bridled and tufted titmice. Ornis Scand., 14, 16–23.
Gahr M ., Leitner S ., Fusani L . & Rybak F . 2002. What is the adaptive role of neurogenesis in adult birds? Progr. Brain Res., 138, 233–54.
Gammon D. E ., Baker M. C . & Tipton J. R . 2005. Cultural divergence within novel song in the black-capped-chickadee (Poecile atricapillus). Auk, 122, 853–71.
Garamszegi L. Z . & Eens M . 2004. Brain space for a learned task: strong intraspecific evidence for neural correlates of singing behavior in songbirds. Brain Res. Rev., 44, 187–93.
Garamszegi L. Z ., Balsby T. J. S ., Bell B. D ., et al. 2005. Estimating the complexity of bird song by using the capture-recapture approaches from community ecology. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 57, 305–17.
Garamszegi L. Z ., Boulinier T ., Møller A. P ., et al. 2002. The estimation of size and composition of avian song repertoires. Anim. Behav., 63, 623–30.
Garamszegi L. Z ., Møller A. P . & Erritzoe J . 2003. The evolution of immune defense and song complexity in birds. Evolution, 57, 905–12.
Garamszegi L. Z ., Pavlova D. Z ., Eens M . & Møller A. P . 2007. The evolution of song in female birds in Europe. Behav. Ecol., 18, 86–96.
Gaunt A. S . & Nowicki S . 1998. Sound production in birds: Acoustics and physiology revisited. In: Animal Acoustic Communication: Sound Analysis and Research Methods (ed. S. L. Hopp , M. J. Owren & C. S. Evans ), pp. 291–322. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Gaunt S. L. L ., Baptista L. F ., Sanchez J. E . & Hernandez D . 1994. Song learning as evidenced from song sharing in two hummingbird species (Colibri coruscans and C. thalassinus). Auk, 111, 87–103.
Geberzahn N . & Hultsch H . 2003. Long-time storage of song types in birds: evidence from interactive playbacks. Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B 270, 1085–90.
Geberzahn N ., Hultsch H . & Todt D . 2002. Latent song type memories are accessible through auditory stimulation in a hand-reared songbird. Anim. Behav., 64, 783–90.
Gentner T. Q . & Hulse S. H . 2000. Female European starling preference and choice for variation in conspecific male song. Anim. Behav., 59, 443–58.
Gibbs H. L . 1990. Cultural evolution of male song types in Darwin's medium ground finches, Geospiza fortis . Anim. Behav., 39, 253–63.
Gibson R. M . & Bradbury J. W . 1985. Sexual selection in lekking sage grouse: phenotypic correlates of male mating success. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 18, 117–23.
Gibson R. M . 1989. Field playback of male display attracts females in a lek breeding sage grouse. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 24, 439–43.
Gil D . & Gahr M . 2002. The honesty of bird song: multiple constraints for multiple traits. Trends Ecol. Evol., 17, 133–41.
Gil D ., Graves J. A . & Slater P. J. B . 1999. Seasonal patterns of singing in the willow warbler: evidence against the fertility announcement hypothesis. Anim. Behav., 58, 995–1000.
Gil D ., Leboucher G ., Lacroix A ., Cue R . & Kreutzer M . 2004. Female canaries produce eggs with greater amounts of testosterone when exposed to preferred male song. Horm. Behav., 45, 64–70.
Gill F. B . & Murray B. G . 1972. Song variation in sympatric blue-winged and golden-winged warblers. Auk, 89, 625–43.
Gill S. A ., Vonhof M. J ., Stutchbury B. J. M ., Morton E. S . & Quinn J. S . 2005. No evidence for acoustic mate-guarding in duetting buff-breasted wrens (Thryothorus leucotis). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 57, 557–65.
Gish S. L . & Morton E. S . 1981. Structural adaptations to local habitat acoustics in Carolina Wren songs. Z. Tierpsychol., 56, 74–84.
Godard R . 1991. Long-term memory of individual neighbours in a migratory songbird. Nature, 350, 228–9.
Godard R . 1993. Tit for tat among neighboring hooded warblers, Wilsonia citrina . Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 33, 45–50.
Goldman S. A . & Nottebohm F . 1983. Neuronal production, migration and differentiation in a vocal control nucleus of the adult canary brain. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA, 80, 2390–4.
Goller F . & Larsen O. N . 1997. A new mechanism of sound generation in songbirds. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 94, 14787–91.
Goller F . & Larsen O. N . 2002. New perspectives on mechanisms of sound generation in songbirds. J. comp. Physiol, A 188, 841–50.
Gonzalez C . & Ornelas J. F . 2005. Song structure and microgeographic song variation in wedge-tailed sabrewings (Campylopterus curvipennis) in Veracruz, Mexico. Auk, 122, 593–607.
Goodale E . & Katagama S. W . 2006b. Vocal mimicry by a passerine bird attracts other species involved in mixed-species flocks. Anim. Behav., 72, 471–477.
Goodale E . & Kotagama S. W . 2006a. Context dependent vocal mimicry in a passerine bird. Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B 273, 875–80.
Goodfellow D. J . & Slater P. J. B . 1986. A model of bird song dialects. Anim. Behav., 34, 1579–80.
Goodfellow D. J . & Slater P. J. B . 1990. Can chaffinches change songs from year to year? Bioacoustics, 2, 249–51.
Goransson G ., Hogstedt G ., Karlsson J ., Kallander H . & Ulfstrand S . 1974. Sangensroll fur revirhallandet hos naktergal Luscinia luscinia – nagra experiment med playback-teknik. Var Fagelvarld 33, 201–9.
Grafe T. U . & Bitz J. H . 2004. Functions of duetting in the tropical boubou, Laniarius aethiopicus: territorial defence and mutual mate guarding. Anim. Behav., 68, 193–201.
Grafe T. U ., Bitz J. H . & Wink M . 2004. Song repertoire and duetting behaviour of the tropical boubou, Laniarius aethiopicus . Anim. Behav., 68, 181–91.
Grant B. R . & Grant P. R . 1979. Darwin's finches: population variation and sympatric speciation. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA, 76, 2359–63.
Grant B. R . & Grant P. R . 1996. Cultural inheritance of song and its role in the evolution of Darwin's finches. Evolution, 50, 2471–87.
Grant B. R . 1984. The significance of song variation in a population of Darwin's finches. Behaviour, 89, 90–116.
Grant P. R . 1986. Ecology and evolution of Darwin's finches. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Greenewalt C.H . 1968. Bird Song: Acoustics and Physiology. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution Press.
Greig-Smith P. W . 1982a. Seasonal patterns of song production by male stonechats Saxicola torquata . Ornis Scand., 13, 225–31.
Greig-Smith P. W . 1982b. Song rates and parental care by individual male stonechats Saxicola torquata . Anim. Behav., 30, 245–52.
Griessmann B . & Naguib M . 2002. Song sharing in neighboring and non-neighboring thrush nightingales (Luscinia luscinia) and its implications for communication. Ethology, 108, 377–87.
Grimes L. G . 1974. Dialects and geographical variation in the song of the splendid sunbird Nectarinia coccinigaster . Ibis, 116, 314–329.
Groschupf K . & Mills G. S . 1982. Singing behavior of the five-striped sparrow. Condor, 84, 226–36.
Groschupf K . 1985. Changes in five-striped sparrow song in intra- and intersexual contexts. Wilson Bull., 97, 102–5.
Gurney M. E . & Konishi M . 1980. Hormone-induced sexual differentiation of brain and behaviour in zebra finches. Science, 208, 1380–3.
Gwinner E . & Dorka V . 1965. Beobachtungen an Zilpzalp-Fitis-Mischsängern. Vogelwelt, 86, 146–51.
Hafner D. J . & Petersen K. E . 1985. Song dialects and gene flow in the white-crowned sparrow, Zonotrichia leucophrys nuttalli . Evolution, 39, 687–94.
Hahnloser R. H ., Kozhevnikov A. A . & Fee M. S . 2002. An ultrasparse code underlies the generation of neural sequences in a songbird. Nature, 419, 65–70.
Hall M. L . & Magrath R. D . 2000. Duetting and mate-guarding in Australian magpie-larks (Grallina cyanoleuca). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 47, 180–7.
Hall M. L . 2004. A review of hypotheses for the functions of avian duetting. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 55, 415–30.
Hall M. L . 2006. Convergent vocal strategies of males and females are consistent with a cooperative function of duetting in Australian magpie-larks. Behaviour, 143, 425–49.
Hall M. L ., Illes A . & Vehrencamp S. L . 2006. Overlapping signals in banded wrens: long-term effects of prior experience on males and females. Behav. Ecol., 17, 260–9.
Halliday T. R . 1983. Information and communication. In: Animal Behaviour, vol. 2: Communication, (ed. T. R. Halliday & P. J. B. Slater ), pp. 43–81. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications.
Hamilton W. D . & Zuk M . 1982. Heritable true fitness and bright birds: a role for parasites? Science, 218, 384–7.
Handford P . & Lougheed S. C . 1991. Variation in duration and frequency characters in the song of the rufous-collared sparrow, Zonotrichia capensis, with respect to habitat, trill dialects and body size. Condor, 93, 644–58.
Handford P . 1981. Vegetational correlates of variation in the song of Zonotrichia capensis . Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 8, 203–6.
Handford P . 1988. Trill rate dialects in the rufous-collared sparrow, Zonotrichia capensis, in northwestern Argentina. Can. J. Zool., 66, 2658–70.
Hansen P . 1979. Vocal learning: its role in adapting sound structures to long-distance propagation and a hypothesis on its evolution. Anim. Behav., 27, 1270–1.
Hansen P . 1981. Coordinated singing in neighbouring yellowhammers (Emberiza citrinella). Natura Jutlandica, 19, 121–38.
Hansen P . 1999. Long term stability of song elements in the yellowhammer Emberiza citrinella . Bioacoustics, 9, 281–95.
Harbison H ., Nelson D. A . & Hahn T. P . 1999. Long-term persistence of song dialects in the mountain white-crowned sparrow. Condor, 101, 133–48.
Harcus J. L . 1977. The functions of mimicry in the vocal behaviour of the chorister robin. Z. Tierpsychol., 44, 178–93.
Harris C. M . 1966. Absorption of sound in air versus humidity and temperature. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 40, 148–59.
Hartley R. S . 1990. Expiratory muscle activity during song production in the canary. Respir. Physiol., 81, 177–88.
Hartshorne C . 1956. The monotony threshold in singing birds. Auk, 83, 176–92.
Hartshorne C . 1973. Born to Sing. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
Harvey P. H . & Pagel M. D . 1991. The Comparative Method in Evolutionary Biology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Hasselquist D ., Bensch S . & von Schantz T . 1996. Correlation between male song repertoire, extra-pair paternity and offspring survival in the great reed warbler. Nature, 381, 229–32.
Hatchwell B. J ., Ross D. J ., Fowlie M. K . & McGowan A . 2001. Kin discrimination in cooperatively breeding long-tailed tits. Proc. R. Soc. Lond, B 268, 885–90.
Hausberger M . 1993. How studies on vocal communication in birds contribute to a comparative approach of cognition. Ethologia, 3, 171–85.
Hausberger M ., Jenkins P. F . & Keene J . 1991. Species-specificity and mimicry in bird song: are they paradoxes? A reevaluation of song mimicry in the European starling. Behaviour, 117, 53–81.
Heinemann D . 1981. Song dialects, migration, and population structure of Puget Sound white-crowned sparrows. Auk, 98, 512–21.
Helb H.-W ., Dowsett-Lemaire F ., Bergmann H.-H . & Conrads K . 1985. Mixed singing in European songbirds - a review. Z. Tierpsychol., 69, 27–41.
Henwood K . & Fabrick A . 1979. A quantitative analysis of the dawn chorus: temporal selection for communicatory optimization. Am. Nat., 114, 260–74.
Hernandez A. M . & MacDougall-Shackleton S. A . 2004. Effects of early song experience on song preferences and song control and auditory brain regions in female house finches (Carpodacus mexicanus). J. Neurobiol., 59, 247–58.
Hessler N. A . & Doupe A. J . 1999. Singing-related neural activity in a dorsal forebrain basal ganglia circuit of adult zebra finches. J. Neurosci., 19, 10461–81.
Heuwinkel H . 1990. The effect of vegetation on the transmission of songs of selected European Passeriformes. Acta Biol. Benrodis, 2, 133–50.
Hiebert S. M ., Stoddard P. K . & Arcese P . 1989. Repertoire size, territory acquisition and reproductive success in the song sparrow. Anim. Behav., 37, 266–73.
Hiett J. C . & Catchpole C. K . 1982. Song repertoires and seasonal song in the yellowhammer Emberiza citrinella . Anim. Behav., 30, 568–74.
Highsmith R. T . 1989. The singing behavior of golden-winged warblers. Wilson Bull., 101, 36–50.
Hile A. G . & Striedter G. F . 2000. Call convergence within groups of female budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus). Ethology, 106, 1105–14.
Hile A. G ., Plummer T. K . & Striedter G. F . 2000. Male vocal imitation produces call convergence during pair bonding in budgerigars, Melopsittacus undulatus . Anim. Behav., 59, 1209–18.
Hill K. G ., Lewis D. B ., Hutchings M. E . & Coles R. B . 1980. Directional hearing in the Japanese quail (Coturnix coturnix japonica). I. Acoustic properties of the auditory system. J. exp. Biol., 86, 135–51.
Hinde R. A . & Steele E . 1976. The effect of male song on an oestrogen-dependent behaviour in the female canary Serinus canarius . Horm. Behav., 7, 293–304.
Hinde R. A . 1958. Alternative motor patterns in chaffinch song. Anim. Behav., 6, 211–18.
Hindmarsh A. M . 1984. Vocal mimicry in starlings. Behaviour, 90, 302–24.
Hindmarsh A. M . 1986. The functional significance of vocal mimicry in song. Behaviour, 99, 87–100.
Hjorth I . 1976. The divalent origin and adaptive radiation of grouse songs. Ornis Scand., 7, 147–57.
Hoelzel A. R . 1986. Song characteristics and response to playback of male and female robins Erithacus rubecula . Ibis, 128, 115–27.
Hoese W. J ., Podos J ., Boetticher N. C . & Nowicki S . 2000. Vocal tract function in birdsong production: experimental manipulation of beak movements. J. exp. Biol., 203, 1845–55.
Holland J ., McGregor P. K . & Rowe C. L . 1996. Changes in microgeographic song variation in the corn bunting. J. Avian Biol., 27, 47–55.
Hooker T . & Hooker B. I . 1969. Duetting. In: Bird Vocalizations (ed. R. A. Hinde ), pp. 185–205. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Horn A . & Falls J. B . 1988a. Structure of Western meadowlark (Sturnella neglecta) song repertoires. Can. J. Zool., 66, 284–8.
Horn A. G . & Falls J. B . 1988b. Repertoires and countersinging in western meadowlarks. Ethology, 77, 337–43.
Horn A. G . & Falls J. B . 1988c. Responses of western meadowlarks, Sturnella neglecta, to song repetition and contrast. Anim. Behav., 36, 291–3.
Hough G. E ., Nelson D. A . & Volman S. F . 2000. Re-expression of songs deleted during vocal development in white-crowned sparrows, Zonotrichia leucophrys . Anim. Behav., 60, 279–87.
Houston A. I . & McNamara J. M . 1987. Singing to attract a mate: a stochastic dynamic game. J. theor. Biol., 129, 57–68.
Houtman A. M . 1992. Female zebra finches choose extra-pair copulations with genetically attractive males. Proc. R. Soc. Lond, B 249, 3–6.
Houx B. B . & ten Cate C . 1999. Song learning from playback in zebra finches: is there an effect of operant contingency? Anim. Behav. 57, 837–45.
Houx B. B ., ten Cate C . & Feuth E . 2000. Variations in zebra finch song copying: an examination of the relationship with tutor song quality and pupil behaviour. Behaviour, 137, 1377–89.
Howard E . 1920. Territory in Bird Life. London: Murray.
Howard R. D . 1974. The influence of sexual selection and interspecific competition on mocking bird song (Mimus polyglottos). Evolution, 28, 428–38.
Huber S. K . & Podos J . 2006. Beak morphology and song production covary in a population of Darwin's finches. Biol. J. Linn. Soc., 88, 489–98.
Hughes M ., Nowicki S ., Searcy W. A . & Peters S . 1998. Song-type sharing in song sparrows: implications for repertoire function and song learning. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 42, 437–46.
Hulse S. H . 2002. Auditory scene analysis in animal communication. Adv. Study Behav., 31, 163–200.
Hulse S. H ., MacDougall-Shackleton S. A . & Wisniewski A. B . 1997. Auditory scene analysis by songbirds: stream segregation of bird song by European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). J. comp. Psychol., 111, 3–13.
Hultsch H . & Kopp M.-L . 1989. Early auditory learning and song improvisation in nightingales, Luscinia megarhynchos . Anim. Behav., 37, 510–512.
Hultsch H . & Todt D . 1982. Temporal performance roles during vocal interactions in nightingales (Luscinia megarhynchos). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 11, 253–60.
Hultsch H . & Todt D . 1989a. Context memorization in the song learning of birds. Naturwissenschaften, 76, 584–6.
Hultsch H . & Todt D . 1989b. Memorization and reproduction of songs in nightingales: evidence for package formation. J. comp. Physiol, A. 165, 197–203.
Hultsch H . & Todt D . 1992. The serial order effect in the song acquisition of birds: relevance of exposure frequency to song models. Anim. Behav., 44, 590–2.
Hultsch H . 1991. Early experience can modify singing styles: evidence from experiments with nightingales, Luscinia megarhynchos . Anim. Behav., 42, 883–9.
Hunter M. L . & Krebs J. R . 1979. Geographical variation in the song of the great tit (Parus major) in relation to ecological factors. J. Anim. Ecol., 48, 759–85.
Hunter M. L . 1989. Himalayan birds face uphill while singing. Auk, 106, 728–9.
Hunter M. L ., Kacelnik A ., Roberts J . & Vuillermoz M . 1986. Directionality of avian vocalizations: a laboratory study. Condor, 88, 371–5.
Hutchinson J. M. C ., McNamara J. M . & Cuthill I. C . 1993. Song, sexual selection, starvation and strategic handicaps. Anim. Behav., 45, 1153–77.
Hyman J . & Hughes M . 2006. Territory owners discriminate between aggressive and nonaggressive neighbours. Anim. Behav., 71, 209–15.
Immelmann K . 1963. Drought adaptations in Australian desert birds. Proc. XIII Int. Orn Congr., 2, 649–57.
Immelmann K . 1969. Song development in the zebra finch and other estrildid finches. In: Bird Vocalizations (ed. R. A. Hinde ), pp. 61–74. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Ince S. A . & Slater P. J. B . 1985. Versatility and continuity in the songs of thrushes Turdus spp. Ibis, 127, 355–64.
Ince S. A ., Slater P. J. B . & Weismann C . 1980. Changes with time in the songs of a population of chaffinches. Condor, 82, 285–90.
Irwin D. E . & Irwin J. H . 2004. Speciation in a ring: the role of song. In Nature's Music: The Science of Birdsong (ed. P. Marler & H. Slabbekoorn ), p. 204. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Irwin D. E . 2000. Song variation in an avian ring species. Evolution, 54, 998–1010.
Irwin D. E ., Bensch S . & Price T. D . 2001. Speciation in a ring. Nature, 409, 333–7.
Irwin R. E . 1990. Directional selection cannot explain variation in song repertoire size in the New World blackbirds (Icterinae). Ethology, 85, 212–24.
Isler M. L ., Isler P. R . & Brumfield R. T . 2005. Clinal variation in vocalisations of an antbird (Thamnophilidae) and implications for defining species limits. Auk, 122, 433–44.
Janik V. M . & Slater P. J. B . 2000. The different roles of social learning in vocal communication. Anim. Behav., 60, 1–11.
Janik V. M . 1999. Pitfalls in the categorization of behaviour: a comparison of dolphin whistle classification methods. Anim. Behav., 57, 133–43.
Jarvi T ., Radesater T . & Jakobsson S . 1980. The song of the willow warbler Phylloscopus trochilus with special reference to singing behaviour in agonistic situations. Ornis Scand., 11, 236–42.
Jarvis E. D . & Nottebohm F . 1997. Motor-driven gene expression. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 94, 4097–102.
Jarvis E. D . 2004. Brains and birdsong. In Nature's Music: The Science of Birdsong (ed. P. Marler & H. Slabbekoorn ), pp. 229–275. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Jarvis E. D ., Smith V. A ., Wada K ., et al. 2002. A framework for integrating the songbird brain. J. comp. Physiol, A 188, 961–80.
Jenkins P. F . & Baker A. J . 1984. Mechanisms of song differentiation in introduced populations of chaffinches Fringilla coelebs in New Zealand. Ibis, 126, 510–24.
Jenkins P. F . 1978. Cultural transmission of song patterns and dialect development in a free-living bird population. Anim. Behav., 26, 50–78.
Jilka A . & Leisler B . 1974. Die Einpassung dreier Rohrsängerarten (Acrocephalus schoenabaenus, A. scirpaceus, A. arundinaceus) in ihre Lebensräume in bezug auf das Frequenzspektrum ihrer Reviergesänge. J. Orn., 115, 192–212.
Jin H . & Clayton D.F . 1997. Localized changes in immediate early gene regulation during sensory and motor learning in zebra finches. Neuron, 19, 1049–59.
Johnson L. S . & Searcy W. A . 1996. Female attraction to male song in house wrens (Troglodytes aedon). Behaviour, 133, 357–66.
Johnson L. S . 1987. Pattern of song type use for territorial defence in the plain titmouse Parus inornatus . Ornis Scand., 18, 24–32.
Jones A. E . & Slater P. J. B . 1993. Do young male zebra finches prefer to learn songs that are familiar to females with which they are housed? Anim. Behav., 46, 616–17.
Jones A. E . & Slater P. J. B . 1996. The role of aggression in song tutor choice in the zebra finch: cause or effect? Behaviour, 133, 103–15.
Jones A. E ., ten Cate C . & Bijleveld C. C. J. H . 2001. The interobserver reliability of scoring sonagrams by eye: a study on methods, illustrated on zebra finch songs. Anim. Behav., 62, 791–801.
Jones A. E ., ten Cate C . & Slater P. J. B . 1996. Early experience and plasticity of song in adult male zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata). J. comp. Psychol., 110, 354–69.
Jouventin P . & Aubin T . 2002 Acoustic systems are adapted to breeding ecologies: individual recognition in nesting penguins. Anim. Behav., 64, 747–57.
Jouventin P ., Aubin T . & Lengagne T . 1999 Finding a parent in a king penguin colony: the acoustic system of individual recognition. Anim. Behav., 57, 1175–83.
Jouventin P ., Guillotin M . & Cornet A . 1979. Le chant du manchot empereur et sa signification adaptive. Behaviour, 70, 231–50.
Kacelnik A . & Krebs J. R . 1983. The dawn chorus in the great tit (Parus major): proximate and ultimate causes. Behaviour, 83, 287–309.
Kelsey M. G . 1989. A comparison of the song and territorial behaviour of a long-distance migrant, the marsh warbler Acrocephalus palustris, in summer and winter. Ibis, 131, 403–414.
Kempenaers B ., Verheyen G. R . & Dhondt A. A . 1997. Extrapair paternity in the blue tit Parus caeruleus: female choice, male characteristics and offspring quality. Behav. Ecol., 8, 481–92.
Kempenaers B ., Verheyen G. R ., van den Broeck M ., Burke T ., van Broeckhoven C . & Dhondt A.A . 1992. Extra-pair paternity results from female preference for high quality males in the blue tit. Nature, 357, 494–96.
Khayutin S. N . 1985. Sensory factors in the behavioral ontogeny of altricial birds. Adv. Study Behav., 15, 105–52.
Kilner R. M ., Noble D. G . & Davies N.B . 1999. Signals of need in a parent-offspring communication and their exploitation by the common cuckoo. Nature, 397, 667–72.
King A. P . & West M. J . 1977. Species identification in the North American cowbird: Appropriate responses to abnormal song. Science, 195, 1002–4.
King A. P . & West M. J . 1983. Epigenesis of cowbird song – a joint endeavour of males and females. Nature, 305, 704–6.
King J. R . 1972. Variation in the song of the rufous collared sparrow, Zonotrichia capensis, in northwestern Argentina. Z. Tierpsychol., 30, 344–73.
Kipper S ., Mundry R ., Hultsch H . & Todt D . 2004. Long-term persistence of performance rules in nightingales (Luscinia megarhynchos): a longitudinal field study on repertoire size and composition. Behaviour, 141, 371–90.
Kirn J. R . & DeVoogd T. J . 1989. The genesis and death of vocal control neurons during sexual differentiation in the zebra finch. J. Neurosci., 9, 3176–87.
Kirn J. R ., Alvarez-Buylla A . & Nottebohm F . 1991. Production and survival of projection neurons in a forebrain vocal centre of adult male canaries. J. Neurosci., 11, 1756–62.
Kirn J. R ., Clower R. P ., Kroodsma D. E . & DeVoogd T. J . 1989. Song-related brain regions in the red-winged blackbird are affected by sex and season but not repertoire size. J. Neurobiol., 20, 139–63.
Kleeman P. M . & Gilardi J. D . 2005. Geographical variation of St Lucia parrot flight vocalizations. Condor, 107, 62–8.
Klump G. M . & Shalter M. D . 1984. Acoustic behaviour of birds and mammals in the predator context. Z. Tierpsychol., 66, 189–226.
Klump G. M ., Kretzschmar E . & Curio E . 1986. The hearing of an avian predator and its avian prey. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 18, 317–23.
Konishi M . & Nottebohm F . 1969. Experimental studies in the ontogeny of avian vocalizations. In: Bird Vocalizations (ed. R. A. Hinde ), pp. 29–48. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Konishi M . 1963. The role of auditory feedback in the vocal behavior of the domestic fowl. Z. Tierpsychol., 20, 349–67.
Konishi M . 1964. Effects of deafening on song development in two species of juncos. Condor, 66, 85–102.
Konishi M . 1965a. Effects of deafening in song development in American robins and black-headed grosbeaks. Z. Tierpsychol., 22, 584–99.
Konishi M . 1965b. The role of auditory feedback in the control of vocalisation in the white-crowned sparrow. Z. Tierpsychol., 22, 770–8.
Konishi M . 1970. Comparative neurophysiological studies of hearing and vocalization in songbirds. Z. vergl. Physiol., 66, 257–72.
Kopuchian C ., Lijtmaer D. A ., Tubaro P. L . & Handford P . 2004. Temporal stability and change in a microgeographical pattern of song variation in the rufous-collared sparrow. Anim. Behav., 68, 551–9.
Kramer H. G . & Lemon R. E . 1983. Dynamics of territorial singing between neighboring song sparrows. Behaviour, 85, 198–223.
Krams I . 2001. Perch selection by singing chaffinches: a better view of surroundings and the risk of predation. Behav. Ecol., 12, 295–300.
Krebs J. R . & Dawkins R . 1984. Animal signals: mind-reading and manipulation. In: Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach (ed. J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies ), pp. 380–402. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications.
Krebs J. R . & Kroodsma D. E . 1980. Repertoires and geographical variation in bird song. Adv. Study Behav., 11, 143–77.
Krebs J. R . 1976. Habituation and song repertoires in the great tit. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 1, 215–27.
Krebs J. R . 1977a. The significance of song repertoires: the Beau Geste hypothesis. Anim. Behav., 25, 475–8.
Krebs J. R . 1977b. Song and territory in the great tit. In: Evolutionary Ecology, (ed. B. Stonehouse & C. Perrins ), pp. 47–62. New York: Macmillan.
Krebs J. R . Davies N. B . 1993. An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications.
Krebs J. R ., Ashcroft R . & van Orsdol K . 1981a. Song matching in the great tit Parus major L. Anim. Behav., 29, 918–23.
Krebs J. R ., Ashcroft R . & Webber M . 1978. Song repertoires and territory defence in the great tit. Nature, 271, 539–42.
Krebs J. R ., Avery M . & Cowie R.J . 1981b. Effect of removal of mate on the singing behaviour of great tits. Anim. Behav., 29, 635–7.
Kriner E . & Schwabl H . 1991. Control of winter song and territorial aggression of female robins (Erithacus rubecula) by testosterone. Ethology, 87, 37–44.
Kroodsma D . 2004. The diversity and plasticity of birdsong. In: Nature's Music: The Science of Birdsong (ed. P. Marler & H. Slabbekoorn ), pp. 111–34. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Kroodsma D. E . & Baylis J. R . 1982. A world survey of evidence for vocal learning in birds. In: Acoustic Communication in Birds (ed. D. E. Kroodsma & E. H. Miller ), pp. 311–37. New York: Academic Press.
Kroodsma D. E . & Byers B. E . 1991. The function(s) of bird song. Amer. Zool., 31, 318–28.
Kroodsma D. E . & Canady R.A . 1985. Differences in repertoire size, singing behaviour and associated neuroanatomy among marsh wren populations have a genetic basis. Auk, 102, 439–46.
Kroodsma D. E . & Konishi M . 1991. A suboscine bird (eastern phoebe, Sayornis phoebe) develops normal song without auditory feedback. Anim. Behav., 42, 477–87.
Kroodsma D. E . & Parker L. D . 1977. Vocal virtuosity in the brown thrasher. Auk, 94, 783–5.
Kroodsma D. E . & Pickert R . 1980. Environmentally dependent sensitive periods for avian vocal learning. Nature, 288, 477–9.
Kroodsma D. E . & Verner J . 1987. Use of song repertoires among marsh wren populations. Auk, 104, 63–72.
Kroodsma D. E . 1978a. Aspects of learning in the ontogeny of birdsong. In: The Development of Behavior: Comparative and Evolutionary Aspects (ed. G. M. Burghardt & M. Bekoff ), pp. 215–30. New York: Garland.
Kroodsma D. E . 1978b. Continuity and versatility in bird song: support for the monotony-threshold hypothesis. Nature, 274, 681–3.
Kroodsma D. E . 1982. Song repertoires: Problems in their definition and use. In: Acoustic Communication In Birds (ed. D. E. Kroodsma & E. H. Miller ), pp. 125–46. New York: Academic Press.
Kroodsma D. E . 1984. Songs of the alder flycatcher (Empidonax alnorum) and willow flycatcher (Empidonax traillii) are innate. Auk, 101 13–24.
Kroodsma D. E . 1985. Development and use of two song forms by the Eastern phoebe. Wilson Bull., 97, 21–9.
Kroodsma D. E . 1986. Design of song playback experiments. Auk, 103, 640–2.
Kroodsma D. E . 1988. Song types and their use: developmental flexibility of the male blue-winged warbler. Ethology, 79, 235–47.
Kroodsma D. E . 1989a. Male eastern phoebes (Sayornis phoebe; Tyrannidae, Passeriformes) fail to imitate songs. J. comp. Psychol., 103, 227–32.
Kroodsma D. E . 1989b. Suggested experimental designs for song playbacks. Anim. Behav., 37, 600–9.
Kroodsma D. E . 1990a. Patterns in songbird singing behaviour: Hartshorne vindicated. Anim. Behav., 39, 994–6.
Kroodsma D. E . 1990b. Using appropriate experimental designs for intended hypotheses in ‘song’ playbacks, with examples for testing effects of song repertoire sizes. Anim. Behav., 40, 1138–50.
Kroodsma D. E . 2005. The Singing Life of Birds. New York: Houghton Mifflin.
Kroodsma D. E ., Bereson R. C ., Byers B. E . & Minear E . 1989. Use of song types by the chestnut-sided warbler: evidence for both intra- and inter-sexual functions. Can. J. Zool., 67, 447–56.
Kroodsma D. E ., Byers B. E ., Goodale E ., Johnson S . & Liu W.-C . 2001. Pseudoreplication in playback experiments, revisited a decade later. Anim. Behav., 61, 1029–33.
Kroodsma D. E ., Houlihan P. W ., Fallon P. A . & Wells J. A . 1997. Song development in grey catbirds. Anim. Behav., 54, 457–64.
Kroodsma D. E ., Liu W.-C ., Goodwin E . & Bedell P. A . 1999a. The ecology of song improvisation as illustrated by North American sedge wrens. Auk, 116, 373–86.
Kroodsma D. E ., Sanchez J ., Stemple D. W ., Goodwin E ., Silva M. L. d . & Vielliard J. M. E . 1999b. Sedentary lifestyle of neotropical sedge wrens promotes song imitation. Anim. Behav., 57, 855–63.
Kroodsma D. E ., Woods R. W . & Goodwin E. A . 2002. Falkland island sedge wrens (Cistothorus platensis) imitate rather than improvise large song repertoires. Auk, 119, 523–8.
Kroodsma D.E . 1976. Reproductive development in a female songbird: differential stimulation by quality of male song. Science, 192, 574–5.
Kroodsma D.E . 1977. Correlates of song organization among North European wrens. Am. Nat., 111, 995–1008.
Kunc H. P ., Amrhein V . & Naguib M . 2005. Seasonal variation in dawn song characteristics in the common nightingale. Anim. Behav., 70, 1265–71.
Kunc H. P ., Amrhein V . & Naguib M . 2006. Vocal interactions in nightingales, Luscinia megarhynchos: more aggressive males have higher pairing success. Anim. Behav., 72, 25–30.
Lachlan R. F . & Servedio M. R . 2004. Song learning accelerates allopatric speciation. Evolution, 58, 2049–63.
Lachlan R. F . & Slater P. J. B . 1999. The maintenance of vocal learning by gene-culture interaction: the cultural trap hypothesis. Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B 266, 701–6.
Lachlan R. F . & Slater P. J. B . 2003. Song learning in chaffinches: how accurate, and from where? Anim. Behav., 65, 957–69.
Lack D . 1946. The Life of the Robin. London: Witherby.
Laiolo P . & Tella J. L . 2005. Habitat fragmentation affects culture transmission: patterns of song-matching in Dupont's lark. J. Appl. Ecol., 42, 1183–93.
Lambrechts M . & Dhondt A . 1986. Male quality, reproduction and survival in the great tit Parus major . Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 19, 57–63.
Lambrechts M . & Dhondt A. A . 1987. Differences in singing performance between male great tits. Ardea, 75, 43–52.
Lambrechts M . & Dhondt A. A . 1988. The anti-exhaustion hypothesis: a new hypothesis to explain song performance and song switching in the great tit. Anim. Behav., 36, 327–34.
Lambrechts M . 1988. Great tit song output is determined by both motivation and by constraints in singing ability: a reply to Weary et al. Anim. Behav., 36, 1244–6.
Lampe H. M . & Espmark Y. O . 1987. Singing activity and song pattern of the redwing Turdus iliacus during the breeding season. Ornis Scand., 18, 179–85.
Lampe H. M . 1991. The response of male redwings Turdus iliacus to playback of conspecific songs with or without the terminating twitter. Ornis Scand., 22, 137–42.
Lang A. L . & Barlow J. C . 1997. Cultural evolution in the Eurasian tree sparrow: divergence between introduced and ancestral populations. Condor, 99, 413–23.
Langmore N. E . & Davies N. B . 1997. Female dunnocks use vocalizations to compete for males. Anim. Behav., 53, 881–90.
Langmore N. E . 1998. Functions of duet and solo songs of female birds. Trends Ecol. Evol., 13, 136–40.
Langmore N. E ., Davies N. B ., Hatchwell B. J . & Hartley I. R . 1996. Female song attracts males in the alpine accentors Prunella collaris . Proc. R. Soc. Lond, B 263, 141–6.
Larom D ., Garstang M ., Payne K ., Raspet R . & Lindeque M . 1997. The influence of surface atmospheric conditions on the range and area reached by animal vocalizations. J. exp. Biol., 200, 421–31.
Larsen O. N . & Dabelsteen T . 1990. Directionality of blackbird vocalization. Implications for vocal communication and its further study. Ornis Scand., 21, 37–45.
Larsen O. N . & Goller F . 1999. Role of syringeal vibrations in bird vocalizations. Proc. R. Soc. Lond, B 266, 1609–15.
Larsen O. N . & Goller F . 2002. Direct observation of syringeal muscle function in songbirds and a parrot. J. exp. Biol., 205, 25–35.
Larsen O. N . 2004. Does the environment constrain avian sound localization? An. Acad. Bras. Ciênc., 76, 267–73.
Latruffe C ., McGregor P. K ., Tavares J. P . & Mota P. G . 2000. Microgeographic variation in corn bunting (Miliaria calandra) song: quantitative and discrimination aspects. Behaviour, 137, 1241–55.
Lauay C ., Gerlach N. M ., Adkins-Regan E . & DeVoogd T. J . 2004. Female zebra finches require early song exposure to prefer high-quality song as adults. Anim. Behav., 68, 1249–55.
Leader N ., Wright J . & Yom-Tov Y . 2000. Microgeographic song dialects in the orange-tufted sunbird (Nectarinia osea). Behaviour, 137, 1613–27.
Leader N ., Wright J . & Yom-Tov Y . 2005. Acoustic properties of two urban song dialects in the orange-tufted sunbird (Nectarinia osea). Auk, 122, 231–45.
Leger D. W . & Mountjoy D. J . 2003. Geographic variation in song of the bright-rumped attila (Tyrannidae: Attila spadiceus): implications for species status. Auk, 120, 69–74.
Lehtonen L . 1983. The changing song patterns of the great tit Parus major . Ornis Fenn., 60, 16–21.
Lein M. R . 1972. Territorial and courtship songs of birds. Nature, 237, 48–9.
Lein M. R . 1978. Song variation in a population of chestnut-sided warblers (Dendroica pensylvanica): Its nature and suggested significance. Can. J. Zool., 56, 1266–83.
Leitão A ., van Dooren T. J. M . & Riebel K . 2004. Temporal variation in chaffinch Fringilla coelebs song: interrelations between the trill and flourish. J. Avian Biol., 35, 199–203.
Leitner S . & Catchpole C. K . 2002. Female canaries that respond and discriminate more between male songs of different quality have a larger song control nucleus (HVC) in the brain. J. Neurobiol., 52, 294–301.
Leitner S . & Catchpole C. K . 2004. Syllable repertoire and the size of the song control system in captive canaries (Serinus canaria) . J. Neurobiol., 60, 21–7.
Leitner S ., Marshall R. C ., Leisler B . & Catchpole C. K . 2006. Male song quality, egg size and offspring sex in captive canaries (Serinus canaria) . Ethology, 112, 554–63.
Leitner S ., Nicholson J ., Leisler B ., DeVoogd T. J . & Catchpole C. K . 2002. Song and the song control pathway in the brain can develop independently of exposure to song in the sedge warbler. Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B 269, 2519–24.
Leitner S ., Voigt C ., Garcia-Segura L ., Van't Hof T . & Gahr M . 2001. Seasonal activation of song motor memories in wild canaries is not reflected in neuroanatomical changes of forebrain song areas. Horm. Behav., 40, 160–8.
Leitner S ., Voigt C ., Metzdorf R . & Catchpole C. K . 2005. Immediate early gene (ZENK, Arc) expression in the auditory forebrain of female canaries varies in response to male song quality. J. Neurobiol., 64, 275–84.
Lemon R. E . & Chatfield C . 1971. Organization of song in cardinals. Anim. Behav., 19, 1–17.
Lemon R. E . 1968. The relation between organization and function of song in cardinals. Behaviour, 32, 158–78.
Lemon R. E . 1974. Song dialects, song matching and species recognition by cardinals Richmondena cardinalis . Ibis, 116, 545–8.
Lemon R. E . 1975. How birds develop song dialects. Condor, 77, 385–406.
Lemon R. E ., Cotter R ., MacNally R. C . & Monette S . 1985. Song repertoires and song sharing by American redstarts. Condor, 87, 457–70.
Lemon R. E ., Dobson C. W . & Clifton P. G . 1993. Songs of American redstarts (Setophaga ruticilla): sequencing rules and their relationships to repertoire size. Ethology, 93, 198–210.
Lemon R. E ., Falls J. B ., Dickinson T ., Perreault S . & Tittler R . 2000. Song clustering by meadowlarks: is it related to repertoire size? Behaviour, 137, 75–92.
Lemon R. E ., Fieldes M. A . & Struger J . 1981a. Testing the monotony threshold of bird song. Z. Tierpsychol., 56, 359–79.
Lemon R. E ., Monette S . & Roff D . 1987. Song repertoires of American warblers (Parulinae): Honest advertising or assessment? Ethology, 74, 265–84.
Lemon R. E ., Perreault S . & Weary D. M . 1994. Dual strategies of song development in American redstarts (Setophaga ruticilla). Anim. Behav., 47, 317–29.
Lemon R. E ., Struger J ., Lechowicz M. J . & Norman R. F . 1981b. Song features and singing heights of American warblers: maximization or optimization of distance? J. Acoust. Soc. Amer., 69, 1169–76.
Leonard M. L . & Horn A. G . 2001. Begging calls and parental feeding decisions in tree swallows, Tachycineta bicolor . Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 49, 170–5.
Leonard M. L ., Horn A. G ., Brown C. R . & Fernandez N. J . 1997. Parent-offspring recognition in tree swallows, Tachycineta bicolor . Anim. Behav., 54, 1107–16.
Leonardo A . & Konishi M . 1999. Decrystallization of adult birdsong by perturbation of auditory feedback. Nature, 399, 466–70.
Leppelsack H. J . & Vogt M . 1976. Responses of auditory neurons in the forebrain of a songbird to stimulation with species-specific sounds. J. comp. Physiol., 107, 263–74.
Levin R. N . 1996a. Song behaviour and reproductive strategies in a duetting wren, Thryothorus nigricapillus. I. Removal experiments. Anim. Behav., 52, 1093–106.
Levin R. N . 1996b. Song behaviour and reproductive strategies in a duetting wren, Thryothorus nigricapillus. II. Playback experiments. Anim. Behav., 52, 1107–17.
Lewis B . & Coles R . 1980. Sound localization in birds. Trends Neurosci., 3, 102–5.
Li X. C ., Jarvis E. D ., Alvarez-Borda B ., Lim D. A . & Nottebohm F . 2000. A relationship between behavior, neurotrophin expression and new neuron survival. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 97, 8584–9.
Lind H ., Dabelsteen T . & McGregor P. K . 1996. Female great tits can identify mates by song. Anim. Behav., 52, 667–71.
Lindell C . 1998. Limited geographical variation in the vocalizations of a neotropical funariid, Synallaxis albescens . Wilson Bull., 110, 368–74.
Liu W.-C . 2004. The effects of neighbours and females on dawn and daytime singing behaviours by male chipping sparrows. Anim. Behav., 68, 39–44.
Logan C. A . 1983. Reproductively dependent song cyclicity in mated male mockingbirds Mimus polyglottos . Auk, 100, 404–13.
Logan C. A ., Hyatt L. E . & Gregorcyk L . 1990. Song playback initiates nest building during clutch overlap in mockingbirds, Mimus polyglottos . Anim. Behav., 39, 943–53.
Logue D. M . & Gammon D. E . 2004. Duet song and sex roles during territory defence in a tropical bird, the black-bellied wren, Thryothorus fasciatoventris . Anim. Behav., 68, 721–31.
Logue D. M . 2006. The duet code of the female back-bellied wren. Condor, 108, 326–35.
Lougheed S. C . & Handford P . 1992. Vocal dialects and the structure of geographic variation in morphological and allozymic characters in the rufous-collared sparrow, Zonotrichia capensis . Evolution, 46, 1443–56.
Lougheed S. C ., Handford P . & Baker A. J . 1993. Mitochondrial DNA hyperdiversity, vocal dialects and subspecies in the rufous-collared sparrow (Zonotrichia capensis). Condor, 95, 889–95.
Lougheed S. C ., Lougheed A. J ., Rae M . & Handford P . 1989. Analysis of a dialect boundary in chaco vegetation in the rufous-collared sparrow. Condor, 91, 1002–05.
Lovell S. F . & Lein M. R . 2004. Neighbor-stranger discrimination by song in a suboscine bird, the alder flycatcher, Empidonax alnorum . Behav. Ecol., 15, 799–804.
Lovell S. F . & Lein M. R . 2005 Individual recognition of neighbors by song in a suboscine bird, the alder flycatcher Empidonax alnorum . Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 57, 623–30.
Luschi P . 1993. Improvisation of new notes during singing by male Sardinian warblers. Bioacoustics, 4, 235–44.
Lynch A . & Baker A. J . 1986. Congruence of morphometric and cultural evolution in Atlantic island chaffinch populations. Can. J. Zool., 64, 1576–80.
Lynch A . & Baker A. J . 1993. A population memetics approach to cultural evolution in chaffinch song: meme diversity within populations. Am. Nat., 141, 597–620.
Lynch A . & Baker A. J . 1994. A population memetics approach to cultural evolution in chaffinch song: differentiation among populations. Evolution, 48, 351–9.
Lynch A ., Plunkett G. M ., Baker A. J . & Jenkins P. F . 1989. A model of cultural evolution of chaffinch song derived with the meme concept. Am. Nat., 133, 634–53.
Møller A. P . 1986. On song post selection and the timing of song in the corn bunting (Miliaria calandra). Ökol. Vögel, 8, 57–66.
Møller A. P . 1988. Spatial and temporal distribution of song in the yellowhammer Emberiza cintrinella . Ethology, 78, 321–31.
Møller A. P . 1991. Why mated songbirds sing so much: mate guarding and male announcement of mate fertility status. Am. Nat., 138, 994–1014.
Møller A. P ., Henry P. Y . & Erritzoe J . 2000. The evolution of song repertoires and immune defence in birds. Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B 267, 165–9.
Møller A. P ., Saino N ., Taramino G ., Galeotti P . & Ferrario S . 1998. Paternity and multiple signalling: effects of a secondary sexual character and song on paternity in the barn swallow. Am. Nat. 151, 236–42.
MacDonald I. F ., Kempster B ., Zanette L . & MacDougall-Shackleton S. A . 2006. Early nutritional stress impairs development of a song control brain region in both male and female juvenile song sparrows. Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B 273, 2559–64.
MacDougall-Shackleton E . & Harbison H . 1998. Singing behavior of lekking green hermits. Condor, 100, 149–52.
MacDougall-Shackleton E . & MacDougall-Shackleton S. A . 2001. Cultural and genetic evolution in mountain white-crowned sparrows: song dialects are associated with population structure. Evolution, 55, 2568–75.
MacDougall-Shackleton S. A ., Hulse S. H . & Ball G. F . 1998. Neural correlates of singing behaviour in male zebra finches Taeniopygia guttata . J. Neurobiol., 36, 421–30.
Mace R. H . 1986. Importance of female behaviour in the dawn chorus. Anim. Behav., 34, 621–2.
Mace R. H . 1987a. The dawn chorus in the great tit Parus major is related to female fertility. Nature, 330, 745–6.
Mace R. H . 1987b. Why do birds sing at dawn. Ardea, 75, 123–32.
Mace R. H . 1989. The relationship between daily routines of singing and foraging: an experiment on captive great tits Parus major . Ibis, 131, 415–20.
MacNally R. C . & Lemon R. E . 1985. Repeat and serial singing modes in American redstarts (Setophaga ruticilla): a test of functional hypotheses. Z. Tierpsychol., 69, 191–202.
Madden J. R . & Davies N. B . 2006. a host-race difference in begging calls of nestling cuckoos Cuculus canorus develops through experience and increases host provisioning. Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B 273, 2343–51.
Malacarne G ., Cucco M . & Camanni S . 1991. Coordinated visual displays and vocal duetting in different ecological situations among Western Palearctic non-passerine birds. Ethol. Ecol. Evol., 3, 207–19.
Mann N. I . & Slater P. J. B . 1994. What causes young male zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata, to choose their father as song tutor? Anim. Behav., 47, 671–7.
Mann N. I ., Dingess K. A . & Slater P. J. B . 2005. Antiphonal four-part synchronized chorusing in a Neotropical wren. Biol. Lett., 2, 1–4.
Mann N. I ., Marshall-Ball L . & Slater P. J. B . 2003. The complex song duet of the plain wren. Condor, 105, 672–82.
Mann N. I ., Slater P. J. B ., Eales L. A . & Richards C . 1991. The influence of visual stimuli on song tutor choice in the zebra finch, Taeniopygia guttata . Anim. Behav., 42, 285–93.
Margoliash D . & Fortune E. S . 1992. Temporal and harmonic combination-sensitive neurons in the zebra finch's HVC. J. Neurosci., 12, 4309–26.
Margoliash D . 1983. Acoustic parameters underlying the responses of song-specific neurons in the white-crowned sparrow. J. Neurosci., 3, 1039–57.
Margoliash D . 1986. Preference for autogenous song by auditory neurons in a song system nucleus of the white-crowned sparrow. J. Neurosci., 6, 1643–61.
Marler P . & Nelson D . 1992. Neuroselection and song learning in birds: species universals in culturally transmitted behavior. Seminars in the Neurosciences, 4, 415–23.
Marler P . & Peters S . 1977. Selective vocal learning in a sparrow. Science, 198, 519–21.
Marler P . & Peters S . 1981. Sparrows learn adult song and more from memory. Science, 213, 780–2.
Marler P . & Peters S . 1982a. Developmental overproduction and selective attrition: new processes in the epigenesis of bird song. Dev. Psychobiol., 15, 369–78.
Marler P . & Peters S . 1982b. Structural change in song ontogeny in the swamp sparrow. Auk, 99, 446–58.
Marler P . & Peters S . 1987. A sensitive period for song acquisition in the song sparrow, Melospiza melodia: a case of age-limited learning. Ethology, 76, 89–100.
Marler P . & Peters S . 1988a. Sensitive periods for song acquisition from tape recordings and live tutors in the swamp sparrow, Melospiza georgiana . Ethology, 77, 76–84.
Marler P . & Peters S . 1988b. The role of song phonology and syntax in vocal learning preferences in the song sparrow, Melospiza melodia . Ethology, 77, 125–49.
Marler P . & Pickert R . 1984. Species-universal microstructure in a learned birdsong: the swamp sparrow (Melospiza georgiana). Anim. Behav., 32, 673–89.
Marler P . & Sherman V . 1983. Song structure without auditory feedback: emendations of the auditory template hypothesis. J. Neurosci., 3, 517–31.
Marler P . & Tamura M . 1964. Song “dialects” in three populations of white-crowned sparrows. Science, 146, 1483–6.
Marler P . 1952. Variations in the song of the chaffinch, Fringilla coelebs . Ibis, 94, 458–72.
Marler P . 1955. Characteristics of some animal calls. Nature, 176, 6–8.
Marler P . 1960. Bird songs and mate selection. In: Animal Sounds and Communication (ed. W. E. Lanyon & W. N. Tavolga ), pp. 348–67. Washington, D.C.: American Institute of Biological Sciences.
Marler P . 1969. Tonal quality in bird sounds. In: Bird Vocalizations (ed. R. A. Hinde ), pp. 5–18. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Marler P . 1970. A comparative approach to vocal learning: song development in white-crowned sparrows. J. comp. physiol. Psychol., 71 (Suppl.), 1–25.
Marler P . 1981. Birdsong: the acquisition of a learned motor skill. Trends Neurosci., 3, 88–94.
Marler P . 1987. Sensitive periods and the roles of specific and general sensory stimulation in birdsong learning. In: Imprinting and Cortical Plasticity (ed. J. P. Rauschecker & P. Marler ), pp. 99–135. New York: Wiley.
Marler P . 1990. Song learning: the interface between behaviour and neuroethology. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B, 329, 109–14.
Marler P . 1991. Song-learning behavior: the interface with neuroethology. Trends Neurosci., 14, 199–206.
Marler P . 2004. Bird calls: a cornucopia for communication. In: Nature's Music: The Science of Birdsong (ed. P. Marler & H. Slabbekoorn ), pp. 135–80. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Marler P ., Peters S ., Ball G. F ., Dufty A. M . & Wingfield J. C . 1988. The role of sex steroids in the acquisition and production of bird song. Nature, 336, 770–2.
Marshall R. C ., Buchanan K. L . & Catchpole C. K . 2003. Sexual selection and individual genetic diversity in a songbird. Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B 270 (Suppl) S248–S250.
Marshall R. C ., Buchanan K. L . & Catchpole C. K . 2007. Song and female choice for extra-pair copulations in the sedge warbler Acrocephalus schoenobaenus . Anim. Behav., 73, 629–35.
Marshall R. C ., Leisler B ., Catchpole C. K . & Schwabl H . 2005. Male song quality affects circulating but not yolk steroid concentrations in female canaries (Serinus canaria) . J. exp. Biol., 208, 4593–8.
Marshall-Ball L . & Slater P. J. B . 2004. Duet singing and repertoire use in threat signaling of the individual and the pair. Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B 271, S440–S443.
Marshall-Ball L ., Mann N. I . & Slater P. J. B . 2006. Multiple functions to duet singing: hidden conflict and apparent cooperation. Anim. Behav., 71, 823–31.
Marten K . & Marler P . 1977. Sound transmission and its significance for animal vocalization. I. Temperate habitats. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 2, 271–90.
Marten K ., Quine D . & Marler P . 1977. Sound transmission and its significance for animal vocalization. II. Tropical forest habitats. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 2, 291–302.
Martens J . & Geduldig G . 1990. Acoustic adaptations of birds living close to Himalayan torrents. In: Proc. Int. 100 Meet. Deutschen Ornithologen-Gesellschaft, pp. 123–131. Bonn.
Martens J . & Kessler P . 2000. Territorial song and song neighbourhoods in the scarlet rosefinch Carpodacus erythrinus . J. Avian Biol., 31, 399–411.
Martens M. J. M . 1980. Foliage as a low pass filter: experiments with model forests in an anechoic chamber. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 67, 66–72.
Martin D. J . 1990. Songs of the fox sparrow. III. Ordering of song. Wilson Bull., 102, 655–71.
Mathevon N ., Aubin T . & Dabelsteen T . 1996. Song degradation during propagation: importance of song post for the wren Troglodytes troglodytes . Ethology, 102, 397–412.
Matyjasiak P . 2004 Birds associate species-specific acoustic and visual cues: recognition of heterospecific rivals by male blackcaps. Behav. Ecol., 16, 467–71.
Maynard Smith J . & Harper D . 2003. Animal Signals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
McCasland J. S . 1987. Neuronal control of bird song production. J. Neurosci., 7, 23–39.
McCracken K. G . & Sheldon F. H . 1997. Avian vocalizations and phylogenetic signal. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 94, 3833–6.
McDonald M. V . 1989. Function of song in Scott's seaside sparrow. Anim. Behav., 38, 468–85.
McGregor P. K . & Avery M. I . 1986. The unsung songs of great tits Parus major: learning neighbours' songs for discrimination. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 18, 311–16.
McGregor P. K . & Dabelsteen T . 1996. Communication networks. In: Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds (ed. D. E. Kroodsma & E. H. Miller ), pp. 409–25. Ithaca, NY, and London: Comstock Publishing Associates.
McGregor P. K . & Falls J. B . 1984. The response of western meadowlarks (Sturnella neglecta) to the playback of undegraded and degraded songs. Can. J. Zool., 62, 2125–28.
McGregor P. K . & Krebs J. R . 1982a. Song types in a population of great tits (Parus major): their distribution, abundance and acquisition by individuals. Behaviour, 79, 126–52.
McGregor P. K . & Krebs J. R . 1982b. Mating and song types in the great tit. Nature, 297, 60–1.
McGregor P. K . & Krebs J. R . 1984a. Song learning and deceptive mimicry. Anim. Behav., 32, 280–7.
McGregor P. K . & Krebs J. R . 1984b. Sound degradation as a distance cue in great tit (Parus major) song. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 16, 49–56.
McGregor P. K . & Thompson D. B. A . 1988. Constancy and change in local dialects of the corn bunting. Ornis Scand., 19, 153–9.
McGregor P. K . (ed) 2005. Animal Communication Networks. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
McGregor P. K . (ed.) 1992a. Playback and Studies of Animal Communication. New York: Plenum Press.
McGregor P. K . 1980. Song dialects in the corn bunting (Emberiza calandra). Z. Tierpsychol., 54, 285–97.
McGregor P. K . 1983. The response of corn buntings to playback of dialects. Z. Tierpsychol., 62, 256–60.
McGregor P. K . 1986. Song types in the corn bunting Emberiza calandra: matching and discrimination. J. Orn., 127, 37–42.
McGregor P. K . 1988. Song length and ‘male quality’ in the chiffchaff. Anim. Behav., 36, 606–8.
McGregor P. K . 1989. Bird song and kin recognition: potential, constraints and evidence. Ethol. Ecol. Evol., 1, 123–7.
McGregor P. K . 1991. The singer and the song: on the receiving end of bird song. Biol. Rev., 66, 57–81.
McGregor P. K . 1992b. Quantifying responses to playback: one, many, or composite multivariate measures. In: Playback and Studies of Animal Communication (ed. P. K. McGregor ), pp. 79–96. New York: Plenum Press.
McGregor P. K . 1993. Signalling in territorial systems: a context for individual identification, ranging and eavesdropping. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B 340, 237–44.
McGregor P. K ., Catchpole C. K ., Dabelsteen T . et al. 1992a. Design of playback experiments: the Thornbridge Hall NATO ARW consensus. In: Playback and Studies of Animal Communication (ed. P. K. McGregor ), pp. 1–9. New York: Plenum Press.
McGregor P. K ., Dabelsteen T ., Shepherd M . & Pedersen S. B . 1992b. The signal value of matched singing in great tits: evidence from interactive playback experiments. Anim. Behav., 43, 987–98.
McGregor P. K ., Krebs J. R . & Perrins C. M . 1981. Song repertoires and lifetime reproductive success in the great tit Parus major . Am. Nat., 118, 149–59.
McGregor P. K ., Krebs J. R . & Ratcliffe L. M . 1983. The reaction of great tits (Parus major) to playback of degraded and undegraded songs: the effect of familiarity with the stimulus song type. Auk, 100, 898–906.
McGregor P. K ., Walford V. R . & Harper D. G. C . 1988. Song inheritance and mating in a songbird with local dialects. Bioacoustics, 1, 107–29.
McNamara J. M ., Mace R. H . & Houston A.I . 1987. Optimal daily routines of singing and foraging in a bird singing to attract a mate. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 20, 399–405.
Medvin M. B . & Beecher M. D . 1986. Parent-offspring recognition in the barn swallow. Anim. Behav., 34, 1627–39.
Mello C. V ., Nottebohm F . & Clayton D . 1995. repeated exposure to one song leads to a rapid and persistent decline in an immediate early gene's response to that song in the zebra finch telencephalon. J. Neurosci., 15, 6919–25.
Mello C. V ., Vicario D. S . & Clayton D. F . 1992. Song presentation induces gene expression in the songbird's forebrain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 89, 6818–22.
Mennill D. J . & Rogers A. C . 2006. Whip it good! Geographic consistency in male songs and variability in female songs of the duetting eastern whipbird Psophodes olicaceus . J. Avian Biol., 37, 93–100.
Mennill D. J . 2006. Aggressive responses of male and female rufous-and-white wrens to stereo duet playback. Anim. Behav., 71, 219–26.
Mennill D. J ., Boag P. T . & Ratcliffe L. M . 2003. The reproductive choices of eavesdropping female black-capped chickadees, Poecile atricapillus . Naturwissenschaften, 90, 577–82.
Mennill D. J ., Ratcliffe L. M . & Boag P. T . 2002. Female eavesdropping on male song contests in songbirds. Science, 296, 873.
Michelsen A . 1983. Biophysical basis of sound communication. In: Bioacoustics: A Comparative Approach (ed. B. Lewis ), pp. 3–38. London: Academic Press.
Miller D. B . 1979a. Long-term recognition of father's song by female zebra finches. Nature, 280, 89–91.
Miller D. B . 1979b. The acoustic basis of mate recognition by female zebra finches (Taeniogpygia guttata). Anim. Behav. 27, 376–80.
Miller E. H . 1982. Character and variance shift in acoustic signals of birds. In: Acoustic Communication in Birds, Vol. 1 (ed. D. E. Kroodsma & E. H. Miller ), pp. 253–95. New York: Academic Press.
Milligan M. M . & Verner J . 1971. Inter-populational song discrimination in the white-crowned sparrow. Condor, 73, 208–13.
Millington S. J . & Price T. D . 1985. Song inheritance and mating patterns in Darwin's finches. Auk, 102, 342–6.
Molles L. E . & Vehrencamp S. L . 1999. Repertoire size, repertoire overlap, and singing modes in the banded wren (Thryothorus pleurostictus). Auk, 116, 677–89.
Molles L. E . & Vehrencamp S. L . 2001 Neighbour recognition by resident males in the banded wren, Thryothorus pleurostictus, a tropical song bird with high song type sharing. Anim. Behav. 61, 119–27.
Molles L. E . & Waas J. R . 2006. Are two heads better than one? Responses of the duetting kokako to one- and two-speaker playback. Anim. Behav., 71, 131–8.
Morrison M. L . & Hardy J. W . 1983. Vocalizations of the black-throated grey warbler. Wilson Bull., 95, 640–3.
Morse D. H . 1966. The context of songs in the yellow warbler. Wilson Bull., 78, 444–55.
Morse D. H . 1970. Territorial and courtship songs of birds. Nature, 226, 659–61.
Morse D. H . 1989. Song patterns of warblers at dawn and dusk. Wilson Bull., 101, 26–35.
Morton E. S . & Derrickson K. C . 1996. Song ranging by the dusky antbird, Cercomacra tyrannina – ranging without song learning. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 39, 195–201.
Morton E. S . 1975. Ecological sources of selection on avian sounds. Am. Nat., 109, 17–34.
Morton E. S . 1986. Predictions from the ranging hypothesis for the evolution of long distance signals in birds. Behaviour, 99, 65–86.
Morton E. S . 1987. The effects of distance and isolation on song-type sharing in the Carolina wren. Wilson Bull., 99, 601–10.
Morton E. S . 1996. Why songbirds learn songs: an arms race over ranging? Poultry & Avian Biology Reviews, 7, 65–71.
Morton E. S . 1998a. Degradation and signal ranging in birds: memory matters. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol ., 42, 135–7.
Morton E. S . 1998b. Reply to Naguib and Wiley. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 42, 147–8.
Morton E. S ., Gish S. L . & van der Voort M . 1986. On the learning of degraded and undegraded songs in the Carolina wren. Anim. Behav., 34, 815–820.
Mountjoy D. J . & Lemon R. E . 1991. Song as an attractant for male and female European starlings, and the influence of song complexity on their response. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 28, 97–100.
Mountjoy D. J . & Lemon R. E . 1995. Extended song learning in wild European starlings. Anim. Behav., 49, 357–66.
Mountjoy D. J . & Lemon R. E . 1996. Female choice for complex song in the European starling: a field experiment. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 38, 65–71.
Mulder R. A ., Bishop H ., Cooper M ., Dennis S ., Koetsveld M ., Marshall J ., Saunders B. L . & Langmore N. E . 2003. Alternate functions for duet and solo songs in magpie-larks, Grallina cyanoleuca . Aust. J. Zool., 51, 25–30.
Mundinger P. C . 1975. Song dialects and colonization in the house finch, Carpodacus mexicanus, on the east coast. Condor, 77, 407–22.
Mundinger P. C . 1982. Microgeographic and macrogeographic variation in the acquired vocalizations of birds. In: Acoustic Communication in Birds (ed. D. E. Kroodsma & E. H. Miller ), pp. 147–208. New York: Academic Press.
Mundry R ., Hau B . & Böhner J . 1994. Individual- und strophenspezifischer Einsatz des “kit” im Gesang des Buchfinken (Fringilla coelebs). J. Orn., 135, 223–31.
Nürnberger F ., Siebold D . & Bergmann H.-H . 1989. Annual changes of learned behaviour – variation of song pattern in free-living chaffinches, Fringilla coelebs, during the breeding season. Bioacoustics, 1, 273–86.
Naguib M . & Kipper S . 2006. Effects of different levels of song overlapping on singing behaviour in male territorial nightingales (Luscinia megarhynchos). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 59, 419–26.
Naguib M . & Todt D . 1997. Effects of dyadic vocal interactions on other conspecific receivers in nightingales. Anim. Behav., 54, 1535–43.
Naguib M . 1996. Ranging by song in Carolina wrens Thyothorus ludovicianus: effects of environmental acoustics and strength of song degradation. Behaviour, 133, 541–59.
Naguib M . 1997. Ranging of songs in Carolina wrens: effects of familiarity of the song type on use of different cues. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 40, 385–93.
Naguib M . 1998. Perception of degradation in acoustic signals and its implications for ranging. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 42, 139–42.
Naguib M . 1999. Effects of song overlapping and alternating on nocturnally singing nightingales. Anim. Behav., 58, 1061–7.
Naguib M ., Altenkamp R . & Griessmann B . 2001. Nightingales in space: song and extra-territorial forays of radio tagged song birds. J. Orn., 142, 306–12.
Naugler C. T . & Ratcliffe L . 1994. Character release in bird song: a test of the acoustic competition hypothesis using American tree sparrows Spizella arborea . J. Avian Biol., 25, 142–8.
Naugler C. T . & Smith P. C . 1991. Song similarity in an isolated population of fox sparrows (Passerella iliaca). Condor, 93, 1001–3.
Nelson D. A . & Marler P . 1990. The perception of bird song and an ecological concept of signal space. In: Comparative Perception, Vol. 2. (ed. W. C. Stebbins & M. A. Berkley ), pp. 443–78. New York: John Wiley.
Nelson D. A . & Marler P . 1993. Innate recognition of song in white-crowned sparrows: a role in selective vocal learning? Anim. Behav., 46, 806–8.
Nelson D. A . 1988. Feature weighting in species song recognition by the field sparrow Spizella pusilla . Behaviour, 106, 158–82.
Nelson D. A . 1989. The importance of invariant and distinctive features in species recognition of bird song. Condor, 91, 120–30.
Nelson D. A . 1992. Song overproduction and selective attrition lead to song sharing in the field sparrow (Spizella pusilla). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 30, 415–424.
Nelson D. A . 1997. Social interaction and sensitive phases for song learning: a critical review. In: Social Influences on Vocal Development (ed. C. T. Snowdon & M. Hausberger ), pp. 7–22. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Nelson D. A . 1998. External validity and experimental design: the sensitive phase for song learning. Anim. Behav., 56, 487–91.
Nelson D. A ., Khanna H . & Marler P . 2001. Learning by instruction or selection: Implications for patterns of geographic variation in bird song. Behaviour, 138, 1137–60.
Nelson D. A ., Marler P . & Morton M. L . 1996. Overproduction in song development: an evolutionary correlate with migration. Anim. Behav., 51, 1127–40.
Nelson D. A ., Marler P . & Palleroni A . 1995. A comparative approach to vocal learning: intraspecific variation in the learning process. Anim. Behav., 50, 83–97.
Nelson D. A ., Marler P ., Soha J. A . & Fullerton A. L . 1997. The timing of song memorization differs in males and females: a new assay for avian vocal learning. Anim. Behav., 54, 587–97.
Nicholls J. A . & Goldizen A. W . 2006. Habitat type and density influence vocal signal design in satin bowerbirds. J. Anim. Ecol., 75. (In press.)
Nicholson J ., Buchanan K. L ., Marshall R. C . & Catchpole C. K . 2007. Song sharing and repertoire size in the sedge warbler Acrocephalus schoenobaenus: changes within and between years. Anim. Behav. (In press.)
Nicolai J . 1959. Familientradition in der Gesangsentwicklung des Gimpels (Pyrrhula pyrrhula L.). J. Orn., 100, 39–46.
Nolan P. M . & Hill G. E . 2004. Female choice for song characteristics in the house finch. Anim. Behav. 67, 403–10.
Nordby J. C ., Campbell S. E . & Beecher M. D . 1999. Ecological correlates of song learning in song sparrows. Behav. Ecol., 10, 287–97.
Nordby J. C ., Campbell S. E . & Beecher M. D . 2002. Adult song sparrows do not alter their song repertoires. Ethology, 108, 39–50.
Nordby L. C ., Campbell S. E . & Beecher M. D . 2001. Late song learning in song sparrows. Anim. Behav., 61, 835–46.
Nordeen K. W . & Nordeen E. J . 1988. Projection neurons within a vocal motor pathway are born during song learning in zebra finches. Nature, 334, 149–51.
Nordeen K. W . & Nordeen E. J . 1992. Auditory feedback is necessary for the maintenance of stereotyped song in adult zebra finches. Behav. Neur. Biol., 57, 58–66.
Nottebohm F . & Arnold A.P . 1976. Sexual dimorphism in vocal control areas of the songbird brain. Science, 194, 211–13.
Nottebohm F . & Nottebohm M. E . 1971. Vocalizations and breeding behaviour of surgically deafened ring doves. Anim. Behav., 19, 313–27.
Nottebohm F . & Nottebohm M. E . 1978. Relationship between song repertoire and age in the canary Serinus canaria . Z. Tierpsychol., 46, 298–305.
Nottebohm F . & Nottebohm M.E . 1976. Left hypoglossal dominance in the control of canary and white-crowned sparrow song. J. comp. Physiol., 108, 171–92.
Nottebohm F . 1968. Auditory experience and song development in the chaffinch Fringilla coelebs . Ibis, 110, 549–68.
Nottebohm F . 1969a. The “critical period” for song learning in birds. Ibis, 111, 386–7.
Nottebohm F . 1969b. The song of the chingolo (Zonotrichia capensis) in Argentina: description and evaluation of a system of dialects. Condor, 71, 299–315.
Nottebohm F . 1971. Neural lateralization of vocal control in a passerine bird. 1 Song. J. exp. Zool., 177, 229–62.
Nottebohm F . 1972. The origins of vocal learning. Am. Nat., 106, 116–40.
Nottebohm F . 1975. Continental patterns of song variability in Zonotrichia capensis: some possible ecological correlates. Am. Nat., 109, 605–24.
Nottebohm F . 1991. Reassessing the mechanisms and origins of vocal learning in birds. Trends Neurosci., 14, 206–11.
Nottebohm F . 2000. The anatomy and timing of vocal learning in birds. In The Design of Animal Communication (ed. M. D. Hauser & M. Konishi ), pp. 63–110. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Nottebohm F . 2002. Why are some neurons replaced in the adult brain? J. Neurosci., 22, 624–28.
Nottebohm F . 2005. The neural basis of birdsong. PLoS Biol., 3(5), e164.
Nottebohm F ., Alvarez-Buylla A ., Cynx J ., et al. 1990. Song learning in birds: the relation between perception and production. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B 329, 115–24.
Nottebohm F ., Kasparian S . & Pandazis C . 1981. Brain space for a learned task. Brain Research, 213, 99–109.
Nottebohm F ., Nottebohm M. E . & Crane L . 1986. Developmental and seasonal changes in canary song and their relation to changes in the anatomy of song control nuclei. Behav. Neural Biol., 46, 445–71.
Nottebohm F ., Nottebohm M. E ., Crane L. A . & Wingfield J. C . 1987. Seasonal changes in gonadal hormone levels of adult male canaries and their relation to song. Behav. Neural Biol., 47, 197–211.
Nottebohm F ., Stokes T. M . & Leonard C. M . 1976. Central control of song in the canary Serinus canarius . J. comp. Neurol., 165, 457–86.
Nowicki S . & Marler P . 1988. How do birds sing? Music Perception, 5, 391–426.
Nowicki S . & Searcy W. A . 2005. Song and mate choice in birds: how the development of behavior helps us understand function. Auk, 122, 1–14.
Nowicki S . 1987. Vocal tract resonances in oscine bird sound production: evidence from bird songs in a helium atmosphere. Nature, 325, 53–5.
Nowicki S ., Marler P ., Maynard A . & Peters S . 1992. Is the tonal quality of bird song learned? Evidence from song sparrows. Ethology, 90, 225–35.
Nowicki S ., Peters S . & Podos J . 1998a. Song learning, early nutrition and sexual selection in songbirds. Amer. Zool., 38, 179–90.
Nowicki S ., Peters S ., Searcy W. A . & Clayton C . 1999. The development of within-song type variation in song sparrows. Anim. Behav., 57, 1257–64.
Nowicki S ., Searcy W. A . & Hughes M . 1998b. The territory defense function of song in song sparrows: a test with the speaker occupation design. Behaviour, 135, 615–28.
Nowicki S ., Searcy W. A . & Peters S . 2002. Brain development, song learning and mate choice in birds: a review and experimental test of the “nutritional stress hypothesis”. J. comp. Physiol. A, 188, 1003–14.
Nuechterlein G. L . 1981. Variations and multiple functions of the advertising display of western grebes. Behaviour, 76, 289–317.
Oberweger K . & Goller F . 2001. The metabolic cost of birdsong production. J. exp. Biol., 204, 3379–85.
Okanoya K . & Dooling R. J . 1988. Hearing in the swamp sparrow Melospiza georgiana, and the song sparrow Melospiza melodia . Anim. Behav., 36, 726–32.
Ornelas J. F ., Gonzalez C . & Uribe J . 2002. Complex vocalizations and aerial displays of the amethyst-throated hummingbird (Lampornis amethystinus). Auk, 119, 1141–9.
Otter K ., Chruszcz B . & Ratcliffe L . 1997. Honest advertisement and song output during the dawn chorus of black-capped chickadees. Behav. Ecol., 8, 167–73.
Otter K ., McGregor P. K ., Terry A. M. R ., et al. 1999. Do female great tits (Parus major) assess males by eavesdropping? A field study using interactive song playback. Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B 266, 1305–9.
Otter K ., Stewart I. R ., McGregor P. K . et al. 2001. Extra-pair paternity among great tits Parus major following manipulation of male signals. J. Avian Biol., 32, 338–44.
Part T . 1991. Is dawn singing related to paternity insurance? The case of the collared flycatcher. Anim. Behav., 41, 451–6.
Paton J. A . & Nottebohm F . 1984. Neurons generated in the adult brain are recruited into functional circuits. Science, 225, 1046–8.
Patricelli G. L ., Dantzker M. S . & Bradbury J. W . 2007. Differences in acoustic directionality among vocalizations of the male red-winged blackbird (Agelaius pheoniceus) are related to function in communication. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 69, 1099–110.
Payne R. B . & Groschupf K. D . 1984. Sexual selection and interspecific competition: a field experiment on territorial behavior of nonparental finches (Vidua spp.). Auk, 101, 140–5.
Payne R. B . & Payne K . 1977. Social organization and mating success in local song populations of village indigobirds Vidua chalybeata . Z. Tierpsychol., 45, 133–73.
Payne R. B . & Payne L. L . 1993. Song copying and cultural transmission in indigo buntings. Anim. Behav., 46, 1045–65.
Payne R. B . 1978a. Local dialects in the wingflaps of flappet larks Mirafra rufocinnamomea . Ibis, 120, 204–7.
Payne R. B . 1978b. Microgeographic variation in songs of splendid sunbirds Nectarinia coccinigaster; population phenetics, habitats and song dialects. Behaviour, 65, 282–308.
Payne R. B . 1981a. Population structure and social behavior: models for testing the ecological significance of song dialects in birds. In: Natural Selection and Social Behavior: Recent Research and New Theory (ed. R. D. Alexander & D. W. Tinkle ), pp. 108–20. New York: Chiron Press.
Payne R. B . 1981b. Song learning and social interaction in indigo buntings. Anim. Behav., 29, 688–97.
Payne R. B . 1982. Ecological consequences of song matching: breeding success and intraspecific song mimicry in indigo buntings. Ecology, 63, 401–11.
Payne R. B . 1983. The social context of song mimicry: song-matching dialects in indigo buntings (Passerina cyanea). Anim. Behav., 31, 788–805.
Payne R. B . 1985. Behavioral continuity and change in local song populations of village indigobirds Vidua chalybeata . Z. Tierpsychol., 70, 1–44.
Payne R. B . 1986. Bird songs and avian systematics. Curr. Ornithol., 87–126.
Payne R. B . 1987. Song dialects and neighborhood habitats in the indigobirds Vidua chalybeata and V. purpurascens at Lochinvar National Park, Zambia. J. Field Ornithol., 58, 152–70.
Payne R. B ., Payne L , & Rowley I . 1988b. Kin and social relationships in splendid fairy wrens: recognition by song in a cooperative bird. Anim. Behav., 36, 1341–51.
Payne R. B ., Payne L. L . & Doehlert S. M . 1988a. Biological and cultural success of song memes in indigo buntings. Ecology, 69, 104–17.
Payne R. B ., Payne L. L . & Woods J. L . 1998. Song learning in brood parasitic indigobirds Vidua chalybeata: song mimicry of the host species. Anim. Behav., 55, 1537–53.
Payne R. B ., Payne L. L ., Woods J. L . & Sorenson M. D . 2000. Imprinting and the origin of parasite-host species associations in brood parasitic indigobirds, Vidua chalybeata . Anim. Behav., 59, 69–81.
Payne R. B ., Thompson W. L ., Fiala K. L . & Sweany L. L . 1981. Local song traditions in indigo buntings: cultural transmission of behavior patterns across generations. Behaviour, 77, 199–221.
Peake T. M . 2005. Eavesdropping in communication networks. In Animal Communication Networks (ed. P. K. McGregor ), pp. 13–37. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Peake T. M ., Otter K. A ., Terry A. M. R . & McGregor P. K . 2000. Screech: an interactive playback program for PCs. Bioacoustics, 11, 69–75.
Peake T. M ., Terry A. M ., McGregor P. K . & Dabelsteen T . 2001. Male great tits eavesdrop on simulated male-to-male vocal interactions. Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B. 268, 1183–7.
Peek F.W . 1972. An experimental study of the territorial function of vocal and visual display in the male red-winged blackbird. Anim. Behav., 20, 112–18.
Pepperberg I. M . 1981. Functional vocalizations by an African grey parrot. Z. Tierpsychol., 55, 139–60.
Pepperberg I. M . 1985. Social modeling theory: a possible framework for understanding avian vocal learning. Auk, 102, 854–64.
Pepperberg I. M . 1990. Learning to communicate: the effects of social interaction. In: Perspectives in Ethology, Volume 9 (ed. P. Bateson & P. H. Klopfer ), pp. 119–64. New York: Plenum.
Pepperberg I. M . 1994. Vocal learning in gray parrots (Psittacus erithacus): effects of social interaction, reference, and context. Auk, 111, 300–13.
Peters S ., Marler P . & Nowicki S . 1992. Song sparrows learn from limited exposure to song models. Condor, 94, 1016–19.
Peters S ., Searcy W. A ., Beecher M. D . & Nowicki S . 2000. Geographic variation in the organization of song sparrow repertoires. Auk, 117, 936–42.
Petrinovich L . & Baptista L. F . 1987. Song development in the white-crowned sparrow: modification of learned song. Anim. Behav., 35, 961–74.
Petrinovich L . & Patterson T. L . 1981. The responses of white-crowned sparrows to songs of different dialects and subspecies. Z. Tierpsychol., 57, 1–14.
Petrinovich L . 1985. Factors influencing song development in the white-crowned sparrow (Zonotrichia leucophrys). J. comp. Psychol., 99, 15–29.
Petrinovich L ., Patterson T . & Baptista L . 1981. Song dialects as barriers to dispersal: a re-evaluation. Evolution, 35, 180–88.
Pinxten R . & Eens M . 1998. Male starlings sing most in the late morning, following egg-laying: a strategy to protect their paternity? Behaviour, 135, 1197–211.
Planck R. J ., McLaren G . & Konishi M . 1975. Temporal pattern shifts in singing birds: a critique. Science, 190, 292.
Podos J . & Nowicki S . 2004. Performance limits on bird song. In Nature's Music: The Science of Birdsong (ed. P. Marler & H. Slabbekoorn ), pp. 322–46. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Podos J . 1996. Motor constraints on vocal development in a songbird. Anim. Behav., 51, 1061–70.
Podos J . 1997. A performance constraint on the evolution of trilled vocalizations in a songbird family (Passeriformes: Emberizidae). Evolution, 5, 537–51.
Podos J . 2001. Correlated evolution of morphology and vocal signature structure in Darwin's finches. Nature, 409, 185–8.
Poesel A . & Dabelsteen T . 2005. Territorial responses of male blue tits to simulated dynamic intrusions: effects of song overlap and intruder location. Anim. Behav., 70, 1419–27.
Poesel A ., Foerster K . & Kempenaers B . 2001. The dawn song of the blue tit Parus caeruleus and its role in sexual selection. Ethology, 107, 521–31.
Poesel A ., Kunc H ., Foerster K ., Johnsen A . & Kempenaers B . 2006. Early birds are sexy: male age, dawn song and extrapair paternity in blue tits, Cyanistes (formerly Parus) caeruleus . Anim. Behav., 72, 531–8.
Popp J. W . & Ficken R.W . 1987. Effects of non-specific singing on the song of the ovenbird. Bird Behav., 7, 22–6.
Popp J. W ., Ficken R. W . & Reinartz J.A . 1985. Short-term temporal avoidance of interspecific acoustic interference among forest birds. Auk, 102, 744–8.
Potash L. M . 1972. Noise-induced changes in calls of the Japanese quail. Psychon. Sci., 26, 252–4.
Prescott D. R.C . 1987. Territorial responses to song playback in allopatric and sympatric populations of alder Empidonax alnorum and willow E. traillii flycatchers. Wilson Bull., 99, 611–19.
Price J. J . & Lanyon S. M . 2002. Reconstructing the evolution of complex bird song in the oropendolas. Evolution, 56, 1514–30.
Price J. J . & Lanyon S. M . 2004. Patterns of song evolution and sexual selection in the oropendolas and caciques. Behav. Ecol., 15, 485–97.
Price J.J . 1999. Recognition of family-specific calls in stripe-backed wrens. Anim. Behav., 57, 483–92.
Putland D. A ., Nicholls J. A ., Noad M. J . & Goldizen A. W . 2006. Imitating the neighbours: vocal dialect matching in a mimic-model system. Biol. Lett., 2, 367–70.
Pytte C. L ., Rusch K. M . & Ficken M. S . 2003. Regulation of vocal amplitude by the blue-throated hummingbird, Lampornis clemenciae . Anim. Behav., 66, 703–10.
Radesater T . & Jakobsson S . 1989. Song rate correlation of replacement territorial willow warblers Phylloscopus trochilus . Ornis. Scand., 20, 71–3.
Radesater T ., Jakobsson S ., Andbjer N ., Bylin A . & Nystrom K . 1987. Song rate and pair formation in the willow warbler Phylloscopus trochilus . Anim. Behav., 35, 1645–51.
Rasmussen R . & Dabelsteen T . 2002. Song repertoires and repertoire sharing in a local group of blackbirds. Bioacoustics, 13, 63–76.
Ratcliffe L . & Weisman R. G . 1986. Song sequence discrimination in the black-capped chickadee Parus atricapillus . J. comp. Psychol., 100, 361–367.
Read A. F . & Weary D. M . 1992. The evolution of bird song: comparative analyses. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond., B 338, 165–87.
Read A. F . & Weary D.M . 1990. Sexual selection and the evolution of bird song: A test of the Hamilton-Zuk hypothesis. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 26, 47–56.
Rechten C . 1978. Interspecific mimicry in bird song: does the Beau Geste hypothesis apply? Anim. Behav., 26, 305.
Reed T.M . 1982. Interspecific territoriality in the chaffinch and the great tit on islands and the mainland of Scotland. Anim. Behav., 30, 171–81.
Reid J. M ., Arcese P ., Cassidy A. L. E. V . et al. 2004. Song repertoire size predicts initial mating success in male song sparrows, Melospiza melodia . Anim. Behav., 68, 1055–63.
Reid J. M ., Arcese P ., Cassidy A. L. E. V . et al. 2005. Hamilton and Zuk meet heterozygosity? Song repertoire size indicates inbreeding and immunity in song sparrows (Melospiza melodia). Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B 272, 481–7.
Reid M. L . 1987. Costliness and reliability in the singing vigour of Ipswich sparrows. Anim. Behav., 35, 1735–43.
Rich T . 1981. Microgeographic variation in the song of the sage sparrow. Condor, 83, 113–19.
Richards D. G . & Wiley R. H . 1980. Reverberations and amplitude fluctuations in the propagation of sound in a forest: implications for animal communication. Am. Nat., 115, 381–99.
Richards D. G . 1981a. Alerting and message components in songs of rufous-sided towhees. Behaviour, 76, 223–49.
Richards D. G . 1981b. Estimation of distance of singing conspecifics by the Carolina wren. Auk, 98, 127–33.
Riebel K . & Slater P. J. B . 1999a. Do male chaffinches Fringilla coelebs copy song sequencing and bout length from their tutors. Ibis, 141, 680–83.
Riebel K . & Slater P. J. B . 1999b. Song type switching in the chaffinch, Fringilla coelebs: timing or counting. Anim. Behav., 57, 655–61.
Riebel K . & Slater P. J. B . 2003. Temporal variation in male chaffinch song depends on the singer and the song type. Behaviour, 140, 269–88.
Riebel K . 2000. Early exposure leads to repeatable preferences for male song in female zebra finches. Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B 267, 2553–8.
Riebel K . 2003a. Developmental influences on auditory perception in female zebra finches – is there a sensitive phase for song preference learning? Anim. Biol., 53, 73–87.
Riebel K . 2003b. The ‘mute’ sex revisited: vocal production and perception learning in female songbirds. Adv. Study Behav., 33, 49–86.
Riebel K ., Smallegange I. M ., Terpstra N. J . & Bolhuis J. J . 2002. Sexual equality in zebra finch song preference: evidence for a dissociation between song recognition and production learning. Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B 269, 729–33.
Ritchison G . 1981. Variation in the songs of vesper sparrows. Am. Midl. Nat., 106, 392–8.
Ritchison G . 1983a. The function of singing in female black-headed grosbeaks (Pheucticus melanocephalus): family group maintenance. Auk, 100, 105–16.
Ritchison G . 1983b. Possible ‘deceptive’ use of song by female black-headed grosbeaks. Condor, 85, 250–1.
Ritchison G . 1985. Variation in the songs of female black-headed grosbeaks. Wilson Bull., 97, 47–56.
Rivers J. W . & Kroodsma D. E . 2000. Singing behavior of the hermit thrush. J. Field Orn., 71, 467–71.
Robinson A . 1949. The biological significance of bird song in Australia. Emu, 48, 291–315.
Robinson F. N . 1974. The function of vocal mimicry in some avian displays. Emu, 74, 9–10.
Robinson F. N . 1975. Vocal mimicry and the evolution of bird song. Emu, 75, 23–7.
Robinson F. N . 1991. Phatic communication in bird song. Emu, 91, 61–3.
Rogers A. C . 2005. Male and female song structure and singing behaviour in the duetting eastern whipbird. Aust. J. Zool., 53, 157–66.
Rogers A. C ., Ferguson J. E ., Harrington H. M ., McDowell S ., Miller A . & Panagos J . 2004. Use of stereo duet playback to investigate traditional duet playback methods and mechanisms of cooperative territorial defence in magpie-larks. Behaviour, 141, 741–53.
Rogers A. C ., Mulder R. A . & Langmore N. E . 2006. Duet duels: sex differences in song matching in duetting eastern whipbirds. Anim. Behav., 72, 53–61.
Rogers D. J . 2003. Geographic song variation within and between populations and subspecies of the rufous bristlebird (Dasyornis broadbenti). Aust. J. Zool., 51, 1–14.
Rost R . 1990. Hormones and behaviour: a joint examination of studies on seasonal variation in song production and plasma levels of testosterone in the great tit Parus major . J. Orn., 131, 403–11.
Rost R . 1992. Hormones and behaviour: a comparison of studies on seasonal changes in song production and testosterone plasma levels in the willow tit Parus montanus . Ornis Fenn., 69, 1–6.
Rost, R. 1987. Entstehung, Fortbestand und funktionelle Bedeutung von Gesangsdialekten bei der Sumpfmeise Parus palustris: ein Test von Modellen. Konstanz: Hartung-Gorre Verlag.
Rothstein S. I . & Fleischer R. C . 1987. Vocal dialects and their possible relation to honest status signalling in the brown-headed cowbird. Condor, 89, 1–23.
Ruppell W . 1933. Physiologie und Akustik der Vogelstimme. J. Orn., 81, 433–542.
Ryan M. J . & Brenowitz E. A . 1985. The role of body size, phylogeny, and ambient noise in the evolution of bird song. Am. Nat., 126, 87–100.
Ryan M. J ., Fox J. H ., Wilczynski W . & Rand A. S . 1990. Sexual selection for sensory exploitation in the frog Physalaemus pustulosus . Nature, 343, 66–7.
Saunders J. C . & Henry W. J . 1989. The peripheral auditory system in birds: structural and functional contributions to auditory perception. In: The Comparative Psychology of Audition: Perceiving Complex Sounds (ed. R. J. Dooling & S. H. Hulse ), pp. 35–6. New Jersey: Erlbaum.
Saunders J. C ., Duncan R. K ., Doan D. E . & Werner Y.L . 2000. The middle ear of reptiles and birds. In: Comparative Hearing: Birds and Reptiles (ed. A. N. Popper ), pp. 13–69. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Schleidt W. M . 1964. Über die Spontaneität von Erbkoordinationen. Z. Tierpsychol., 21, 235–6.
Schroeder D. J . & Wiley R. H . 1983a. Communication with repertoires of song themes in tufted titmice. Anim. Behav., 31, 1128–38.
Schroeder D. J . & Wiley R. H . 1983b. Communication with shared song themes in tufted titmice. Auk, 100, 414–24.
Schwabl H . & Kriner E . 1991. Territorial aggression and song of male European robins (Erithacus rubecula) in autumn and spring: effects of antiandrogen treatment. Horm. Behav., 25, 180–94.
Searcy W. A & Nowicki S . 2005. The Evolution of Animal Communication: Reliability and Deception in Signaling Systems. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Searcy W. A . & Andersson M . 1986. Sexual selection and the evolution of song. Ann. Rev. Ecol. Syst., 17, 507–33.
Searcy W. A . & Brenowitz E. A . 1988. Sexual differences in species recognition of avian song. Nature, 332, 152–4.
Searcy W. A . & Marler P . 1981. A test for responsiveness to song structure and programming in female sparrows. Science, 213, 926–8.
Searcy W. A . & Marler P . 1984. Interspecific differences in the response of female birds to song repertoires. Z. Tierpsychol., 66, 128–42.
Searcy W. A . & Yasukawa K . 1990. Use of the song repertoire in intersexual and intrasexual contexts by male red-winged blackbirds. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 27, 123–8.
Searcy W. A . & Yasukawa K . 1996. Song and female choice. In Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds (ed. D. E. Kroodsma & E. H. Miller ), pp. 454–73. Ithaca, NY, and London: Comstock Publishing Associates.
Searcy W. A . 1988. Dual intersexual and intrasexual functions of song in red-winged blackbirds. Proc. XIX Int. Congr. Ornithol., 1, 1373–81.
Searcy W. A . 1989. Pseudoreplication, external validity and the design of playback experiments. Anim. Behav., 38, 715–17.
Searcy W. A . 1990. Species recognition of song by female red-winged blackbirds. Anim. Behav., 40, 1119–27.
Searcy W. A . 1992a. Song repertoire and mate choice in birds. Amer. Zool., 32, 71–80.
Searcy W. A . 1992b. Measuring responses of female birds to male song. In: Playback and Studies of Animal Communication (ed. P. K. McGregor ), pp. 175–89. New York: Plenum Press.
Searcy W. A ., Coffman S . & Raikow D. F . 1994. Habituation, recovery and the similarity of song types within repertoires in red-winged blackbirds (Agelaius phoeniceus) (Aves, Emberizidae). Ethology, 98, 38–49.
Searcy W. A ., McArthur P. D . & Yasukawa K . 1985. Song repertoire size and male quality in song sparrows. Condor, 87, 222–8.
Searcy W. A ., Nowicki S . & Hughes M . 1997. The response of male and female song sparrows to geographic variation in song. Condor, 99, 651–7.
Searcy W. A ., Nowicki S . & Peters S . 2003. Phonology and geographic song discrimination in song sparrows. Ethology, 109, 23–35.
Searcy W. A ., Nowicki S ., Hughes M . & Peters S . 2002. Geographic song discrimination in relation to dispersal distances in song sparrows. Am. Nat., 159, 221–30.
Searcy W.A . 1984. Song repertoire size and female preferences in song sparrows. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 14, 281–6.
Seddon N . & Tobias J. A . 2003. Communal singing in the cooperatively breeding subdesert mesite Monias benschi: evidence of numerical assessment? J. Avian. Biol., 34, 72–80.
Seddon N . & Tobias J. A . 2006. Duets defend mates in a suboscine passerine, the warbling antbird (Hypocnemis cantator). Behav. Ecol., 17, 73–83.
Seddon N . 2002. The structure, context and possible functions of solos, duets and choruses in the subdesert mesite (Monias benschi). Behaviour, 139, 645–76.
Seddon N . 2005. Ecological adaptation and species recognition drives vocal evolution in neotropical suboscine birds. Evolution, 59, 200–15.
Seddon N ., Butchart S. H. M . & Odling-Smee L . 2002. Duetting in the subdesert mesite Monias benschi: evidence for acoustic mate defence? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 52, 7–16.
Seibt U ., Wickler W ., Kleindienst H.-U . & Sonnenschein E . 2002. Structure, geography and origin of dialects in the traditive song of the forest weaver Ploceus bicolor sclateri in Natal, S. Africa. Behaviour, 139, 1237–65.
Shutler D . & Weatherhead P . 1990. Targets of sexual selection: song and plumage of wood warblers. Evolution, 44, 1967–77.
Shy E . & Morton E. S . 1986. Adaptation of amplitude structure of songs to propagation in field habitat in song sparrows. Z. Tierpsychol., 72, 177–84.
Shy E . 1983. The relation of geographical variation in song to habitat characteristics and body size in North American tanagers (Thraupinae: Piranga). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 12, 71–6.
Simpson B. S . 1984. Tests of habituation to song repertoires by Carolina wrens. Auk, 101, 244–54.
Simpson H. B . & Vicario D. S . 1990. Brain pathways for learned and unlearned vocalizations differ in zebra finches. J. Neurosci., 10, 1541–56.
Slabbekoorn H . & den Boer-Visser A . 2006. Cities change the songs of birds. Curr. Biol. 16, 2326–31.
Slabbekoorn H . & Peet M . 2003. Birds sing at a higher pitch in urban noise. Nature, 424, 267.
Slabbekoorn H . & Smith T. B . 2002a. Bird song, ecology and speciation. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond, B 357, 493–503.
Slabbekoorn H . & Smith T. B . 2002b. Habitat dependent song divergence in the little greenbul: an analysis of environmental selection pressures on acoustic signals. Evolution, 56, 1849–58.
Slabbekoorn H . 2004. Singing in the wild: the ecology of birdsong. In: Nature's Music: The Science of Birdsong (ed. P. Marler & H. Slabbekoorn ), pp. 181–208. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Slabbekoorn H ., Ellers J . & Smith T. B . 2002. Birdsong and sound transmission: the benefits of reverberations. Condor, 104, 564–73.
Slagsvold T . 1977. Bird song activity in relation to breeding cycle, spring weather and environmental phenology. Ornis Scand., 8, 197–222.
Slagsvold T ., Dale S . & Saetre G.-P . 1994. Dawn singing in the great tit (Parus major): mate attraction, mate guarding, or territorial defence? Behaviour 131, 115–38.
Slater P. J. B . & Ince S. A . 1979. Cultural evolution in chaffinch song. Behaviour, 71, 146–66.
Slater P. J. B . & Ince S. A . 1982. Song development in chaffinches: what is learnt and when? Ibis, 124, 21–6.
Slater P. J. B . & Lachlan R. F . 2003. Is innovation in bird song adaptive? In: Animal Innovation (ed. K. N. Laland & S. Reader ), pp. 117–35. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Slater P. J. B . & Mann N. I . 1990. Do male zebra finches learn their fathers' songs? Trends Ecol. Evol., 5, 415–17.
Slater P. J. B . & Mann N. I . 1991. Early experience and song learning in zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata). In: Acta XX Congressus Internationalis Ornithologici, pp. 1074–80. Christchurch, New Zealand: Ornithological Congress Trust Board.
Slater P. J. B . & Mann N. I . 2004. Why do the females of many bird species sing in the tropics? J. Avian Biol., 35, 289–94.
Slater P. J. B . 1978. Beau Geste has problems. Anim. Behav., 26, 304–5.
Slater P. J. B . 1981. Chaffinch song repertoires: observations, experiments and a discussion of their significance. Z. Tierpsychol., 56, 1–24.
Slater P. J. B . 1983a. Bird song learning: theme and variations. In: Perspectives in Ornithology. (ed. A. H. Brush & G. A. Clark ), pp. 475–99. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Slater P. J. B . 1983b. Sequences of song in chaffinches. Anim. Behav., 31, 272–81.
Slater P. J. B . 1983c. The study of communication. In: Animal Behaviour. Vol. 2: Communication (ed. T. R. Halliday & P. J. B. Slater ), pp. 9–42. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications.
Slater P. J. B . 1983d. The development of individual behaviour. In: Animal Behaviour. Vol. 3: Genes, Development and Learning (ed. T. R. Halliday & P. J. B. Slater ), pp. 82–113. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications.
Slater P. J. B . 1986. The cultural transmission of bird song. Trends Ecol. Evol., 1, 94–7.
Slater P. J. B . 1989. Bird song learning: causes and consequences. Ethol. Ecol. Evol., 1, 19–46.
Slater P. J. B ., Clements F. A . & Goodfellow D. J . 1984. Local and regional variations in chaffinch song and the question of dialects. Behaviour, 88, 76–97.
Slater P. J. B ., Eales L. A . & Clayton N. S . 1988. Song learning in zebra finches: progress and prospects. Adv. Study Behav., 18, 1–34.
Slater P. J. B ., Gil D ., Barlow C. R . & Graves J. A . 2002. Male led duets in the moho (Hypergerus atriceps) and yellow-crowned gonolek (Lanarius barbarus): mate guarding by females. Ostrich, 73, 49–51.
Slater P. J. B ., Ince S. A . & Colgan P. W . 1980. Chaffinch song types: their frequencies in the population and distribution between the repertoires of different individuals. Behaviour, 75, 207–18.
Slater P. J. B ., Lachlan R. F . & Riebel K . 2000. The significance of learning in signal development: The curious case of the chaffinch. In: Animal Signals. Signalling and Signal Design in Animal Communication (ed. Y. Espmark , T. Amundsen & G. Rosenqvist ), pp. 341–52. Trondheim, Norway: Tapir Publishers.
Smith D. G . & Norman D. O . 1979. ‘Leader-follower’ singing in red-winged blackbirds. Condor, 81, 83–4.
Smith D. G . 1976. An experimental analysis of the function of red-winged blackbird song. Behaviour, 56, 136–56.
Smith D. G . 1979. Male singing ability and territory integrity in red-winged blackbirds Agelaius phoeniceus . Behaviour, 68, 193–206.
Smith W. J . 1977. The Behavior of Communicating. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Soha J. A . & Marler P . 2000. A species-specific acoustic cue for selective song learning in the white-crowned sparrow. Anim. Behav., 60, 297–306.
Soha J. A ., Nelson D. A . & Parker P. G . 2004. Genetic analysis of song dialect populations in Puget sound white-crowned sparrows. Behav. Ecol., 15, 636–46.
Sohrabji F ., Nordeen E. J . & Nordeen K. W . 1990. Selective impairment of song learning following lesions of a forebrain nucleus in the juvenile zebra finch. Behav. Neural Biol., 53, 51–63.
Sonnenschein E . & Reyer H.-U . 1983. Mate-guarding and other functions of antiphonal duets in the slate-coloured boubou (Laniarius funebris). Z. Tierpsychol., 63, 112–40.
Sorjonen J . & Merilä J . 2000. Response of male bluethroats Luscinia svecica to song playback: evidence of territorial function of song and song flights. Ornis Fennica, 77, 43–7.
Sorjonen J . 1986a. Mixed singing and interspecific territoriality - consequences of secondary contact of two ecologically and morphologically similar nightingale species in Europe. Ornis Scand., 17, 53–67.
Sorjonen J . 1986b. Singing strategies in Northern European passerines. Univ. of Joensu Publications in Sciences, 9, 1–21.
Sorjonen J . 1987. Temporal and spatial differences in traditions and repertoires in the song of the thrush nightingale (Luscinia luscinia). Behaviour, 102, 196–212.
Spector D. A ., McKim L. K . & Kroodsma D. E . 1989. Yellow warblers are able to learn songs and situations in which to use them. Anim. Behav., 38, 723–5.
Speirs E. A . & Davis L. S . 1991. Discrimination by adelie penguins Pygoscelis adeliae, between the loud mutual calls of mates, neighbours and strangers. Anim. Behav., 41, 937–44.
Spencer K. A ., Buchanan K. A ., Goldsmith A. R . & Catchpole C. K . 2003. Song as an honest indicator of developmental history in the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). Horm. Behav., 44, 132–9.
Spencer K. A ., Buchanan K. A ., Leitner S ., Goldsmith A. R . & Catchpole C. K . 2005a. Parasites affect song complexity and neural development in a songbird. Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B 272, 2037–43.
Spencer K. A ., Wimprenny J. H ., Buchanan K. L . et al. 2005b. Developmental stress affects the attractiveness of male song and female choice in the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 58, 423–8.
Staicer C. A . 1989. Characteristics, use, and significance of two singing behaviors in Grace's warbler. Auk, 106, 49–63.
Staicer C. A ., Spector D. A . & Horn A. G . 1996. The dawn chorus and other diel patterns in acoustic signalling. In: Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds (ed. D. E. Kroodsma & E. H. Miller ), pp. 426–53. Ithaca, NY, and London: Comstock Publishing Associates.
Stoddard P. K . & Beecher M. D . 1983. Parent recognition of offspring in the cliff swallow. Auk, 100, 795–9.
Stoddard P. K . 1996. Vocal recognition of neighbours by territorial passerines. In: Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds (ed. D. E. Kroodsma & E. H. Miller ), pp. 356–74. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
Stoddard P. K ., Beecher M. D ., Campbell S. E . & Horning C. L . 1992. Song-type matching in the song sparrow. Can. J. Zool., 70, 1440–4.
Stoddard P. K ., Beecher M. D ., Horning C. L . & Campbell S. E . 1990. Strong neighbour-stranger discrimination in song sparrows. Condor, 92, 1051–6.
Suthers R. A . 1999. The motor basis of vocal performance in songbirds. In The Design of Animal Communication (ed. M. D. Hauser & M. Konishi ), pp. 37–62. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Suthers R. A . 2004. How birds sing and why it matters. In Nature's Music: The Science of Birdsong (ed. P. Marler & H. Slabbekoorn ), pp. 276–99. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Suthers R. A ., Goller F . & Pytte C . 1999. The neuromuscular control of bird song. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B 354, 927–39.
Suthers R. A ., Vallet E ., Tanvez A . & Kreutzer M . 2004. Bilateral song production in domestic canaries. J. Neurobiol., 60, 381–93.
Suthers R.A . 1990. Contributions to bird song from the left and right sides of the intact syrinx. Nature, 347, 473–7.
Szekely T ., Catchpole C. K ., DeVoogd A ., Marchl Z & DeVoogd T. J . 1996. Evolutionary changes in a song control area of the brain (HVC) are associated with evolutionary changes in song repertoire among European warblers (Sylviidae). Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B 263, 607–10.
Tanvez A ., Beguin N ., Chastel O ., Lacroix A . & Leboucher G . 2004. Sexually attractive phrases increase yolk androgens deposition in canaries Serinus canaria . Gen. comp. Endocrinol., 138, 113–20.
Tchernichovski O ., Mitra P. P ., Lints T . & Nottebohm F . 2001. Dynamics of the vocal imitation process: how a zebra finch learns its song. Science, 291, 2564–9.
Tchernichovski O ., Nottebohm F ., Ho C. E ., Pesaran B . & Mitra P. P . 2000. A procedure for automated measurement of song similarity. Anim. Behav., 59, 1167–76.
Temrin H . 1986. Singing behaviour in relation to polyterritorial polygyny in the wood warbler (Phylloscopus sibilatrix). Anim. Behav., 34, 146–52.
ten Cate C ., Slabbekoorn H . & Ballintijn M . 2002. Birdsong and male-male competition: causes and consequences of vocal variability in the collared dove (Streptopelia decaocto). Adv. Study Behav., 31, 31–75.
Thielcke G . & Krome M . 1989. Experimente über sensible Phasen und Gesangsvariabilität beim Buchfinken (Fringilla coelebs). J. Orn., 130, 435–53.
Thielcke G . & Krome M . 1991. Chaffinches Fringilla coelebs do not learn song during autumn and early winter. Bioacoustics, 3, 207–12.
Thielcke G . 1969. Geographic variation in bird vocalizations. In: Bird Vocalizations (ed. R. A. Hinde ), pp. 310–39. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Thielcke G . 1973. On the origin of divergence of learned signals (songs) in isolated populations. Ibis, 115, 511–16.
Thielcke G . 1986. Constant proportions of mixed singers in treecreeper populations (Certhia familiaris). Z. Tierpsychol., 72, 154–64.
Thielcke G . 1987. Langjährige Dialektkonstanz beim Gartenbaumläufer (Certhia brachydactyla). J. Orn., 128, 171–80.
Thielcke G . 1988. Buchfinken (Fringilla coelebs) eliminieren erlernte Gesänge von Baumpiepern (Anthus trivialis). Die Vogelwarte, 34, 319–36.
Thielcke-Poltz H . & Thielcke G . 1960. Akustisches Lernen verschieden alter schallisolierter Amseln (Turdus merula L.) und die Entwicklung erlernter Motive ohne und mit künstlichen Einfluss von Testosteron. Z. Tierpsychol., 17, 211–44.
Thomas R. J . & Cuthill I. C . 2002. Body mass regulation and the daily singing routines of European robins. Anim. Behav. 63, 285–95.
Thomas R. J . 1999a. Two tests of a stochastic dynamic programming model of daily singing routines in birds. Anim. Behav. 57, 277–84.
Thomas R. J . 1999b. The effect of variability in the food supply on the daily singing routines of European robins: a test of a stochastic dynamic programming model. Anim. Behav. 57, 365–9.
Thomas R. J ., Szekely T ., Cuthill I. C ., Harper D. G. C ., Newson S. E ., Frayling T. D . & Wallis P. D . 2002. Eye size in birds and the timing of song at dawn. Proc. R. Soc. Lond, B 269, 831–7.
Thorpe W. H . 1954. The process of song learning in the chaffinch as studied by means of the sound spectrograph. Nature, 173, 465.
Thorpe W. H . 1958. The learning of song patterns by birds, with especial reference to the song of the chaffinch Fringilla coelebs . Ibis, 100, 535–70.
Thorpe W. H . 1961. Bird Song. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Thorpe W. H . 1972. Duetting and antiphonal song in birds, its extent and significance. Behaviour Suppl., 18, 1–197.
Todt D . & Geberzahn N . 2003. Age-dependent effects of song exposure: song crystallization sets a boundary between fast and delayed vocal imitation. Anim. Behav., 65, 971–9.
Todt D . & Hultsch H . 1982. Impairment of vocal signal exchange in the monogamous duet-singer Cossypha heuglini (Turdidae): Effects on pair bond maintenance. Z. Tierpsychol., 60, 265–74.
Todt D . & Hultsch H . 1998. How songbirds deal with large amounts of serial information: retrieval rules suggest a hierarchical song memory. Biol. Cybern., 79, 487–500.
Todt D . & Naguib M . 2000. Vocal interactions in birds: the use of song as a model in communication. Adv. Study Behav., 29, 247–96.
Todt D . 1975a. Short term inhibition of outputs occurring in the vocal behaviour of blackbirds (Turdus m. merula L.). J. comp. Physiol., 98, 289–306.
Todt D . 1975b. Spontaneous recombinations of vocal patterns in parrots. Naturwissenschaften, 62, 399.
Todt D . 1981. On functions of vocal matching: effect of counter-replies on song post choice and singing. Z. Tierpsychol., 57, 73–93.
Todt D ., Hultsch H . & Duvall F. P . 1982. Behavioural significance and social function of vocal and non-vocal displays in the monogamous duet-singer Cossypha heuglini H. Zool. Beitr., 27, 421–48.
Tomback D. F . & Baker M. C . 1984. Assortative mating by white-crowned sparrows at song dialect boundaries. Anim. Behav., 32, 465–9.
Tomback D. F ., Thompson D. B . & Baker M. C . 1983. Dialect discrimination by white-crowned sparrows: reactions to near and distant dialects. Auk, 100, 452–60.
Trainer J. M . & Parsons R. J . 2001. Uniformity of long-tailed manakin songs from three localities in Costa Rica. Wilson Bull., 113, 431–4.
Trainer J. M . 1989. Cultural evolution in song dialects of yellow-rumped caciques in Panama. Ethology, 80, 190–204.
Trainer J. M ., McDonald D. B . & Learn W. A . 2002. The development of coordinated singing in cooperatively displaying long-tailed manakins. Behav. Ecol., 13, 65–9.
Trivers R. L . 1972. Parental investment and sexual selection. In: Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man (ed. B. Campbell ), pp. 137–79. London: Heinemann.
Tschanz B . 1968. Trottellummen. Z. Tierpsychol. Suppl. 4.
Tsipoura N . & Morton E. S . 1988. Song-type distribution in a population of Kentucky warblers. Wilson Bull., 100, 9–16.
Tubaro P. L . & Mahler B . 1998. Acoustic frequencies and body mass in New World doves. Condor, 100, 54–61.
Tubaro P. L . & Segura E. T . 1994. Dialect differences in the song of Zonotrichia capensis in the southern pampas: a test of the acoustic adaptation hypothesis. Condor, 96, 1084–8.
Tubaro P. L ., Segura E. T . & Handford P . 1993. Geographic variation in the song of the rufous-collared sparrow in Eastern Argentina. Condor, 95, 588–95.
Turner W. C . & Barber C. A . 2004. Male song sparrows Melospiza melodia do not announce their female's fertility. J. Avian Biol., 35, 483–6.
Vallet E . & Kreutzer M . 1995. Female canaries are sexually responsive to special song phrases. Anim. Behav., 49, 1603–10.
Vallet E ., Beme I ., & Kreutzer M . 1998. Two-notes syllables in the canary songs elicit high levels of sexual display. Anim. Behav., 55, 291–7.
van Buskirk J . 1997. Independent evolution of song structure and note structure in American wood warblers. Proc. R. Soc. Lond., B 264, 755–61.
Veerman P. A . 1994. Batesian acoustic mimicry by the regent honeyeater Xanthomyza phrygia . Austral. Bird Watcher, 15, 250–9.
Voigt C ., Leitner S . & Gahr M . 2006. Repertoire and structure of duet and solo songs in cooperatively breeding white-browed sparrow weavers. Behaviour, 143, 159–82.
Volman S. F . & Khanna H . 1995. Convergence of untutored song in group-reared zebra finches. J. comp. Psychol., 109, 211–21.
von Haartman L . & von Numers M . 1992. Rain call dialects of the chaffinch Fringilla coelebs in the archipelago of SW Finland. Ornis Fennica, 69, 65–71.
von Helversen D . 1980. Structure and function of antiphonal duets. In: Acta XVII Congr. Int. Orn. Berlin.
Waas J. R . 1988. Song pitch-habitat relationships in white-throated sparrows: cracks in acoustic windows? Can. J. Zool., 66, 2578–81.
Wallin L . 1986. Divergent character displacement in the song of two allospecies: the pied flycatcher Ficedula hypoleuca, and the collared flycatcher Ficedula albicollis . Ibis, 128, 251–9.
Wallschläger D . 1980. Correlation of song frequency and body weight in passerine birds. Experientia, 36, 412.
Wanker R ., Sugama Y . & Prinage S . 2005. Vocal labelling of family members in spectacled parrotlets (Forpus conspicillatus). Anim. Behav., 70, 111–18.
Ward B ., Nordeen E . & Nordeen K . 2001. Anatomical and ontogenetic factors producing variation in HVC neuron number in zebra finches. Brain Res., 904, 318–26.