Drawing and the Non-Verbal Mind

A Life-Span Perspective

Drawing and the Non-Verbal Mind

Drawing and its analysis has been an important discipline of Developmental Psychology since the early twentieth century. This unique collection of essays unites leading empirical researchers from Europe, the United States and Canada to provide a valuable introduction to drawing research. Focusing on the core problems associated with the visual mind, the contributors examine how drawing development relates to changes in cognition. Topics covered include visual (self) recognition, style, media understanding, inhibition, executive attention, priming, memory, meaning, and figural and spatial concepts. The effects of biological constraints such as motor control, grip and handedness, blindness, neuropsychological conditions and old age are also explained. The book provides a fascinating insight into the life-span and productivity of the non-verbal, visual mind.

Adi-Japha, E., Levin, I. and Solomon, S. (1998). Emergence of representation in drawing: the relation between kinematic and referential aspects. Cognitive Development, 13, 23–49.
Agnetta, B. and Rochat, P. (2004). Imitative games by 9-, 14-, and 18-month-old infants. Infancy, 6, 1–36.
Akshoomoff, N. A. and Stiles, J. (1995a). Developmental trends in visuospatial analysis and planning: I. Copying a complex figure. Neuropsychology, 9, 364–77.
Akshoomoff, N. A. and Stiles, J. (1995b). Developmental trends in visuospatial analysis and planning: Ⅱ. Memory for a complex figure. Neuropsychology, 9, 378–89.
Aldhous, P. (2006). Elephants recognize their mirror image. New Scientist, 192, 17.
Alpaugh, P. K. and Birren, J. E. (1977). Variables affecting creative contributions across the adult life span. Human Development, 20, 240–8.
Amsterdam, B. (1972). Mirror self-image reactions before age two. Developmental Psychobiology, 5, 297–305.
Amsterdam, B. (1972). Mirror self-image reactions before the age of two. Developmental Psychobiology, 5, 297–305.
Anati, E. (2002). Höhlenmalerei [Cave paintings]. Düsseldorf: Albatros.
Anderson, J. R. and Gallup, G. G., Jr (1999). Self-recognition in nonhuman primates: past and future challenges. In M. Haug and R. E. Whalen (eds.), Animal models of human emotion and cognition (pp. 175–94). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Antonelli, I. R., Corsonello, A., Pedone, C., Trojano, L., Acanfora, D., Spada, A., Izzo, O. and Rengo, F. (2006). Drawing impairment predicts mortality in severe COPD. Chest, 130, 1687–94.
Arber, A. (1954). The mind and the eye. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Arnheim, R. (1954). Art and visual perception. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Arnheim, R. (1974). Art and visual perception. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Arnheim, R. (1974). Art and visual perception: a psychology of the creative eye. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Asendorpf, J. B. (2002). Self-awareness, other-awareness, and secondary representation. In A. N. Meltzoff and W. Prinz (eds.), The imitative mind: development, evolution, and brain bases (pp. 63–73). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Asendorpf, J. B. and Baudonniere, P. M. (1993). Self-awareness and other-awareness: mirror self-recognition and synchronic imitation among unfamiliar peers. Developmental Psychology, 29, 88–95.
Baddeley, A. D. and Lieberman, K. (1980). Spatial working memory. In R. S. Nickerson (ed.), Attention and performance (Vol. 3, pp. 521–39). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bahrick, L. E. and Watson, J. S. (1985). Detection of intermodal proprioceptive-visual contingency as a potential basis of self-perception in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 21, 963–73.
Baltes, P. B. (1987). Theoretical propositions of life-span developmental psychology: on the dynamics between growth and decline. Developmental Psychology, 23, 611–26.
Baltes, P. B. and Kliegl, R. (1992). Further testing of limits of cognitive plasticity: negative age differences in a mnemonic skill are robust. Developmental Psychology, 28, 121–5.
Bard, K. A. (1998). Social-experiential contributions to imitation and emotion in chimpanzees. In S. Braten (ed.), Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny: a source book (pp. 208–27). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bard, K. A. (2005). Emotions in chimpanzee infants: the value of a comparative developmental approach to understand the evolutionary bases of emotion. In J. Nadel and D. Muir (eds.), Emotional development (pp. 31–60). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Bard, K. A. (2007). Neonatal imitation in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) tested with two paradigms. Animal Cognition, 10, 233–42.
Bard, K. A., Myowa-Yamakoshi, M., Tomonaga, M., Tanaka, M., Costall, A. and Matsuzawa, T. (2005). Group differences in the mutual gaze of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Developmental Psychology, 41, 616–24.
Bard, K. A., Todd, B. K., Bernier, C., Love, J. and Leavens, D. A. (2006). Self-awareness in human and chimpanzee infants: what is measured and what is meant by the mirror-and-mark test? Infancy, 9, 191–219.
Barlow, C. M., Jolley, R. P., White, D. G. and Galbraith, D. (2003). Rigidity in children's drawings and its relation with representational change. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 124–52.
Barlow, C. M., Jolley, R. P., White, D. G. and Galbraith, D. (2003). Rigidity in children's drawings and its relation with representational change. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 124–52.
Barlow, C. M., Jolley, R. P., White, D. G. and Galbraith, D. (2003). Rigidity in children's drawings and its relation with representational change. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 124–52.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1987). Autism and symbolic play. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 139–48.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Are autistic children behaviorists? An examination of their mental–physical and appearance–reality distinctions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 579–600.
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M. and Frith, U. (1986). Mechanical, behavioral and intentional understanding of picture stories in autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 113–25.
Barrett, M. D. and Light, P. H. (1976). Symbolism and intellectual realism in children's drawings. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 46, 198–202.
Barrett, M., Beaumont, A. and Jennett, M. (1985). Some children do sometimes what they have been told to do: task demands and verbal instructions on children's drawings. In N. H. Freeman and M. V. Cox (eds.), Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation (pp. 176–87). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bartel, K. (1927/58). Perspektywa malarska [Painterly perspective]. Warsaw: Panstwowe Wydawnictwo Naukowe.
Barth, J., Povinelli, D. J. and Cant, J. G. H. (2004). Bodily origins of SELF. In D. R. Beike, J. M. Lampinen and D. A. Behrend (eds.), The self and memory (pp. 11–43). New York: Psychology Press.
Baschek, I.-L., Bredenkamp, J., Oehrle, B. and Wippich, W. (1977). Bestimmung der Bildhaftigkeit (I), Konkretheit (C) und der Bedeutungshaltigkeit (m′) von 800 Substantiven [Determination of imageability (I), concreteness (C) and meaningfulness (m′) for 800 nouns]. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie, 24, 353–96.
Baxendall, M. (1985). Patterns of intention. London: Yale University Press.
Baylis, G. C. (1998). Visual parsing and object-based attention: a developmental perspective. In J. E. Richards (ed.), Cognitive neuroscience of attention: a developmental perspective (pp. 251–86). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Baylis, G. C. and Driver, J. (1995). One-sided edge-assignment in vision: 1. Figure–ground segmentation and attention to objects. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 140–6.
Behr, H.-G., Grohmann, H. and Hagedorn, B.-O. (1983). Charakter-Köpfe: Der Fall F. X. Messerschmidt [Heads with character: the case of F. X. Messerschmidt]. Basel and Weinheim: Beltz.
Beilin, H. and Pearlman, E. G. (1991). Children's iconic realism: object versus property realism. In H. W. Reese (ed.), Advances in child development and behaviour (Vol. 23, pp. 73–111). New York: Academic Press.
Bekoff, M. (2002). Animal reflections. Nature, 419, 255.
Bensur, B., Eliot, J. and Hedge, L. (1997). Cognitive correlates of the complexity of children's drawings. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 85, 1079–89.
Berg, I. (2003). Zeichnungen blinder und sehender Kinder im Vergleich [Comparison of drawings by blind and sighted children]. Stuttgart: Ibidem.
Bern, C. and Herzog, H. A. (1994). Stimulus control of defensive behaviours of garter snakes (Thamnophis sirtalis): Effects of eyespots and movement. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108, 353–7.
Bertenthal, B. I. and Fischer, K. W. (1978). Development of self-recognition in the infant. Developmental Psychology, 14, 44–50.
Berti, A. E. and Freeman, N. H. (1997). Representational change in resources for pictorial innovations: a three-component analysis. Cognitive Development, 12, 501–22.
Bigelow, A. E. (1981). The correspondence between self- and image movement as a cue to self-recognition for young children. Journal of Genetic Psycholology, 139, 11–26.
Binet, A. (1890). Perception d'enfant [Children's perception]. Revue Philosophique, 30, 47–72.
Bischof-Kohler, D. (1991).The development of empathy in infants. In M. E. Lamb and H. Keller (eds.), Infant development: perspectives from German-speaking countries (pp. 245–73). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bisiach, E. and Vallar, G. (1988). Hemineglect in humans. In F. Boller and J. Grafman (eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (pp. 195–222). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Blöte, A. W. and Heijden, P. G. M. Van der (1988). A follow-up study on writing posture and writing movement in young children. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 14, 57–74.
Blöte, A. W., Zielstra, E. M. and Zoetewey, M. W. (1987). Writing posture and writing movement in kindergarten. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 13, 323–41.
Bloom, P. and Markson, L. (1998). Intention and analogy in children's naming of pictorial representations. Psychological Science, 9, 200–4.
Bosman, E. A. (1993). Age-related differences in the motoric aspects of transcription typing skill. Psychology and Aging, 8, 283–308.
Bosman, E. A. and Charness, N. (1996). Age-related differences in skilled performance and skill acquisition. In F. Blanchard-Fields and T. M. Hess (eds.), Perspectives on cognitive change in adulthood and aging (pp. 428–53). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Bovet, D. and Vauclair, J. (2000). Picture recognition in animals and in humans: a review. Behavioral Brain Research, 109, 143–65.
Bower, G. H. (1970). Analysis of a mnemonic device. American Scientist, 58, 496–510.
Boysen, S. T., Bryan, K. M. and Shreyer, T. A. (1994). Shadows and mirrors: alternative avenues to the development of self-recognition in chimpanzees. In S. T. Parker, R. W. Mitchell and M. L. Boccia (eds.), Self-awareness in animals and humans: developmental perspectives (pp. 227–40). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Bozeat, S., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Graham, K. S., Patterson, K., Wilkin, H., Rowland, J., Rogers, T. T. and Hodges, J. R. (2003). A duck with four legs: investigating the structure of conceptual knowledge using picture drawing in semantic dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 27–47.
Bozeat, S., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Graham, K. S., Patterson, K., Wilkin, H., Rowland, J., Rogers, T. T. and Hodges, J. R. (2003). A duck with four legs: investigating the structure of conceptual knowledge using picture drawing in semantic dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 27–47.
Braswell, G. S. and Rosengren, K. S. (2000). Decreasing variability in the development of graphic production. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24, 153–66.
Braswell, G. S. and Rosengren, K. S. (2002). Interactions between cognitive and biomechanical factors in drawing development: a glimpse into the role of handedness. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 581–99.
Braswell, G. S., Rosengren, K. S. and Pierroutsakos, S. L. (2007). Task constraints on preschool children's grip configurations during drawing. Developmental Psychobiology, 49, 216–25.
Brehmer, Y., Li, S.-C., Müller, V., Oertzen, T. von and Lindenberger, U. (2007). Memory plasticity across the lifespan: uncovering children's latent potential. Developmental Psychology, 43, 465–78.
Bremner, G. and Moore, S. (1984). Prior visual inspection and object naming: two factors that enhance hidden-feature inclusion in young children's drawings. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2, 371–6.
Bremner, J. G. (1984). Errors towards the perpendicular in children's copies of angular figures: a test of the bisection interpretation. Perception, 13, 117–28.
Bremner, J. G. (1985). Provoked use of height in pictures as a depth cue in young children's drawings. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2, 95–8.
Bremner, J. G. and Batten, A. (1991). Sensitivity to viewpoint in children's drawings of objects and relations between objects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 52, 375–94.
Bremner, J. G., Morse, R., Hughes, S. and Andreasen, G. (2000). Relations between drawing cubes and copying line diagrams of cubes in 7 to 10-years-old children. Child Development, 71, 621–34.
Brooks-Gunn, J. and Lewis, M. (1984). The development of early visual self-recognition. Developmental Review, 4, 215–39.
Bruner, J. S. and Koslowski, B. (1972). Visually preadapted constituents of manipulatory action. Perception, 1, 3–14.
Burkitt, E., Barrett, M. and Davis, A. (2003). Children's color choices for completing drawings of affectively characterised topics. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 445–55.
Cabe, P. A., Wright, C. D. and Wright, M. A. (2003). Descartes’ blind man revisited: bimanual triangulation of distance using static hand-held rods. American Journal of Psychology, 116, 71–98.
Callaghan, T. C. (2000). Factors affecting children's graphic symbol use in the third year. Language, similarity, and iconicity. Cognitive Development, 15, 185–214.
Cannoni, E. (2003). Il disegno dei bambini [The drawings of children]. Rome: Carocci.
Carey, S. and Diamond, R. (1977). From piecemeal to configurational representation of faces. Science, 195, 312–14.
Caron-Pargue, J. (1985). Le dessin du cube chez l'enfant [Children's drawings of cubes]. Berne: Lang.
Case, R. (1995). Capacity-based explanations of working memory growth: a brief history and re-evaluation. In F. E. Weinert and W. Schneider (eds.), Memory performance and competencies: issues in growth and development (pp. 23–44). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Case, R. and Okamoto, Y. (1996). The role of central conceptual structures in the development of children's thought. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development (SRCD), 61, 1–2.
Case, R., Stephenson, K. M., Bleiker, C. and Okamoto, Y. (1996). Central spatial structures and their development. In R. Case and Y. Okamoto (eds.), The role of central conceptual structures in the development of children's thought (pp. 103–30). Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 246. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: their structure, growth, and action. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
Chaloupka, G. (1983). From palaeoart to casual painting. Darwin Northern Territory Museum of Arts and Sciences.
Chapman, M. (1987). A longitudinal study of cognitive representation in symbolic play, self-recognition, and object permanence during the second year. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 10, 151–70.
Charman, T. and Baron-Cohen, S. (1993). Drawing development in autism: the intellectual to visual realism shift. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11, 171–85.
Charness, N. (1981). Aging and skilled problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 21–38.
Charness, N. (1989). Age and expertise: responding to Talland's challenge. In L. W. Poon, D. C. Rubin and B. A. Wilson (eds.), Everyday cognition in adulthood and late life (pp. 437–56). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Chen, M. J. (1985). Young children's representational drawings of solid objects: a comparison of drawing and copying. In N. H. Freeman and M. V. Cox (eds.), Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation (pp. 157–75). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Chen, M. J. and Cook, M. (1984). Representational drawings of solid objects by young children. Perception, 13, 377–85.
Cohen, D. J. and Bennett, S. (1997). Why can't most people draw what they see? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 609–21.
Connolly, K. and Elliott, J. (1972). The evolution and ontogeny of hand function. In N. Blurton Jones (ed.), Ethological studies of child behaviour (pp. 329–84). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Courage, M. L., Edison, S. C. and Howe, M. L. (2004). Variability in the early development of visual self-recognition. Infant Behaviour and Development, 27, 509–32.
Cowan, N., Saults, J. S. and Elliott, E. M. (2002). The search for what is fundamental in the development of working memory. In R. V. Kail and H. W. Reese (eds.), Advances in child development and behaviour (Vol. 29, pp. 1–49). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Cox, M. (1992). Children's drawings. New York: Penguin.
Cox, M. V. (1985). One object behind another: young children's use of array-specific or view-specific representations. In N. H. Freeman and M. V. Cox (eds.), Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation (pp. 188–201). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Cox, M. V. (1985). One object behind another: young children's use of array-specific or view-specific representations. In N. H. Freeman and M. V. Cox (eds.), Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation (pp. 188–281). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Cox, M. V. (1986). Children's drawings. London: Penguin.
Cox, M. V. (1991). The child's point of view: the development of cognition and language. Second edition. London: Harvester Press.
Cox, M. V. (1992). Children's drawings. Harmondsworth: Penguin.
Cox, M. V. (1992). Children's drawings. Harmondsworth: Penguin.
Cox, M. V. (1992). Children's drawings. London: Penguin.
Cox, M. V. (1993). Children's drawings of the human figure. Hove: Erlbaum.
Cox, M. V. (1993). Children's drawings of the human figure. Hove: Erlbaum.
Cox, M. V. (2005). The pictorial world of the child. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Cox, M. V. (2005). The pictorial world of the child. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Cox, M. V. and Perara, J. (1998). Children's observational drawings. A nine-point scale for scoring drawings of a cube. Educational Psychology, 18, 309–17.
Custance, D., Whiten, A. and Bard, K. A. (1995). Can young chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) imitate arbitrary actions? Hayes and Hayes (1952) revisited. Behaviour, 132, 837–59.
D'Angiulli, A., Kennedy, J. M. and Heller, M. A. (1998). Blind children recognizing tactile pictures respond like sighted children given guidance in exploration. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 39, 187–90.
David, F. N. and Barton, D. E. (1962). Combinatorial chance. London: Charles Griffin.
Davies, A. (1985). Conflict between canonicality and array-specificity in young children's drawings. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3, 363–72.
Davies, A. M. (1983). Contextual sensitivity in young children's drawings. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 35, 478–86.
Davis, A. (1985). Conflict between canonicality and array-specificity in young children's drawings. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3, 363–72.
Davis, A. M. (1985). The canonical bias: young children's drawing of familiar objects. In N. H. Freeman and M. V. Cox (eds.), Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation (pp. 202–13). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Davis, A. M. (1985). The canonical bias: young children's drawings of familiar objects. In N. H. Freeman and M. V. Cox (eds.), Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation (pp. 202–13). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
De Bruyn, B. and Davis, A. (2005). Breakdown of parallel spatial coding in children's drawing. Developmental Science, 8, 226–8.
de Ribaupierre, A. and Bailleux, C. (1994). Developmental change in a spatial task of attentional capacity: an essay toward an integration of two working memory models. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 17, 5–35.
de Waal, F. B. M., Dindo, M., Freeman, C. A. and Hall, M. J. (2005). The monkey in the mirror: hardly a stranger. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 102, 11,140–7.
DeLoache, J. S. (1987). Rapid change in the symbolic functioning of very young children. Science, 238, 1556–7.
DeLoache, J. S. (1989). Young children's understanding of the correspondence between a scale model and a larger space. Cognitive Development, 4, 121–39.
DeLoache, J. S. (1991). Symbolic functioning in very young children: understanding of pictures and models. Child Development, 62, 736–52.
DeLoache, J. S. (2000). Dual representation and young children's use of scale models. Child Development, 71, 329–38.
DeLoache, J. S. (2002). Symbolic development. In U. Goswami (ed.), Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 206–26). Oxford: Blackwell.
DeLoache, J. S. (2004). Becoming symbol-minded. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 66–70.
DeLoache, J. S. and Burns, N. M. (1994). Early understanding of the representational function of pictures. Cognition, 52, 83–110.
DeLoache, J. S., Miller, K. F. and Rosengren, K. S. (1997). The credible shrinking room: very young children's performance with symbolic and non-symbolic relations. Psychological Science, 8, 308–13.
DeLoache, J. S., Miller, K. F. and Rosengren, K. S. (1997). The credible shrinking room: very young children's performance with symbolic and nonsymbolic relations. Psychological Science, 8, 308–13.
DeLoache, J. S., Peralta de Mendoza, O. A. and Anderson, K. (1999). Multiple factors in early symbol use: instructions, similarity and age in understanding a symbol–referent relation. Cognitive Development, 14, 299–312.
DeLoache, J. S., Pierroutsakos, S. L., Uttal, D. H., Rosengren, K. S. and Gottlieb, A. (1998). Grasping the nature of pictures. Psychological Science, 9, 205–10.
DeLoache, J. S., Strauss, M. S. and Maynard, J. (1979). Picture perception in infancy. Infant Behaviour and Development, 2, 77–89.
DeLoache, J., Uttal, D. and Rosengren, K. S. (2004). Scale errors offer evidence for a perception–action dissociation early in life. Science, 304, 1027–9.
Dennis, S. (1987). The development of childen's drawing: a neo-structuralist approach. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Toronto.
Dennis, S. (1992). Stage and structure in the development of children's spatial representations. In R. Case (ed.), The mind's staircase (pp. 229–45). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Deppe, C., Holt, D., Tweksbury, J., Broberg, L., Petersen, J. and Wood, K. (2003). Effect of northern pygmy owl (Glaucidium gnoma) eyespots on avian mobbing. The Auk, 120, 765–71.
Deręgowski, J. B. (1976). Coding and drawing of simple geometric stimuli by Bukusu school-children in Kenya. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 7, 195–208.
Deręgowski, J. B. (1984). Distortion in art: the eye and the mind. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Deręgowski, J. B. (1986). Kazimierz Bartel's observations on drawings of children and illiterate adults. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 331–3.
Deręgowski, J. B. (1994). Bartel, K. (1882–1941). Psychologie und Geschichte, 5, 246–60.
Deręgowski, J. B. and Dziurawiec, S. (1994). Perceptual impulsions and unfamiliar solids: evidence from a drawing task. British Journal of Psychology, 85, 1–15.
Deręgowski, J. B. and Dziurawiec, S. (1996). The puissance of typical contours and children's drawings. Australian Journal of Psychology, 48, 98–103.
Deręgowski, J. B. and McGeorge, P. (1998a). The role of typical contours in encodement of objects in three-dimensional arrays. Perception, 27, 283–94.
Deręgowski, J. B. and McGeorge, P. (1998b). Perceived similarity of shapes is an asymmetrical relationship. Perception, 27, 35–46.
Deręgowski, J. B., Parker, D. M. and Dziurawiec, S. (1996). The role of typical contours in object processing by children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 425–40.
Deręgowski, J. B., Shepherd, J. and Slaven, G. A. (1997). Sex differences on Bartel's test. British Journal of Psychology, 88, 637–51.
DeSilva, J. and Lesnik, J. (2006). Chimpanzee neonatal brain size: implications for brain growth in Homo erectus. Journal of Human Evolution, 51, 207–12.
Diamond, A. (1991). Neuropsychological insight into the meaning of object concept development. In S. Carey and R. Gelman (eds.), The epigenesis of mind (pp. 67–110). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Doherty, M. J. and Wimmer, M. (2005). Children's understanding of ambiguous figures: which cognitive developments are necessary to experience reversal? Cognitive Development, 20, 407–21.
Draganski, B., Gaser, C., Busch, V., Schuierer, G., Bogdahn, U. and May, A. (2004). Changes in grey matter induced by training. Nature, 427, 311–12.
Driver, J., Baylis, G. C., Goodrich, S. J. and Rafal, R. D. (1994). Axis-based neglect of visual shapes. Neuropsychologia, 32, 1353–65.
Dukette, D. and Stiles, J. (1996). Children's analysis of hierarchical patterns: evidence from a similarity judgment task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 103–40.
Dunbar, R. (2003). The social brain: mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective. Annual Review of Anthropology, 33, 163–81.
Duval, T. S. and Wicklund, R. A. (1972). A theory of objective self-awareness. New York: Academic Press.
Dziurawiec, S. and Deręgowski, J. B. (1992). Twisted perspective in young children's drawings. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 10, 35–49.
Dziurawiec, S. and Deręgowski, J. B. (2002). The eyes have it: perceptual investigation of eyespots. Perception, 31, 1313–22.
Dziurawiec, S. and Deręgowski, J. B. (2007). The perceptual effect of eyespots, or the devil may seem closer than she really is. Australian Journal of Psychology, 59, 101–7.
Eames, K. and Cox, M. V. (1994). Visual realism in the drawings of autistic, Down's syndrome and normal children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 235–9.
Edelman, G. M. (2006). The embodiment of mind. Daedalus, 135, 23–32.
Edelstein, W., Keller, M. and Schröder, E. (1990). Child development and social structure: a study of individual differences. In R. M. Lerner (ed.), Life-span development and behaviour (pp. 152–87). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Edwards, B. (1992). Drawing on the right side of the brain. London: Souvenir Press.
Efron, R. and Yundt, E. W. (1996). Spatial non-uniformities in visual search. Brain and Cognition, 31, 331–68.
Ekstrom, R. B., French, J. W. and Harman, H. H. (1976). Manual for kit of factor-references cognitive tests. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.
Emile, O., Floch, A. L. and Vollrath, F. (2006). Biopolymers: shape memory in spider draglines. Nature, 440, 621.
Eng, H. (1931). The psychology of children's drawings. New York: Harcourt, Brace.
Engelkamp, J. and Zimmer, H. D. (1997). Sensory factors in memory for subject-performed tasks. Acta Psychologica, 96, 43–60.
Epstein, R., Lansa, R. P. and Skinner, B. F. (1981). ‘Self-awareness’ in the pigeon. Science, 212, 695–6.
Erickson, M. J. (1996). Introduction to combinatorics. New York: Wiley.
Eriksson, Y. (1998). Tactile pictures: pictorial representations for the blind 1784–1940. Goteberg: Goteberg University Press.
Feldman, D. H. (2000). Figurative and operative processes in the development of artistic talent. Human Development, 43, 60–4.
Flavell, J. H., Flavell, E. R. and Green, F. L. (1983). Development of the appearance–reality distinction. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 95–120.
Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L. and Flavell, E. R. (1986). Development of knowledge about the appearance–reality distinction. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 51, 1–87.
Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L. and Flavell, E. R. (1986). Development of knowledge about the appearance–reality distinction. Monographs of the Society for Research on Child Development, 51 (1, serial no. 212).
Flavell, J., Miller, P. and Miller, S. (2002). Cognitive development. Hillsdale, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Foelix, R. F. (1996). Biology of spiders. Second edition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Foley, E. J. and Berch, D. B. (1997). Capacity limitations of a classic M-power measure: a modified dual-task approach. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 66, 129–43.
Fortes, M. (1940). Children's drawings among the Tallensi. Africa, 13, 293–5.
Fortes, M. (1981). Tallensi children's drawings. In B. Lloyd and J. Gay (eds.), Universals of human thought. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Franklin, S., Van Sommers, P. and Howard, D. (1992). Drawing without meaning? Dissociations in the graphic performance of an agnostic artist. In R. Campbell (ed.), Mental lives: case studies in cognition (pp. 178–98). Oxford: Blackwell.
Freeman, N. and Janikoun, R. (1972). Intellectual realism in children's drawings of a familiar object with distinctive features. Child Development, 43, 1116–21.
Freeman, N. H. (1972). Process and product in children's drawing. Perception, 1, 123–40.
Freeman, N. H. (1972). Process and product in children's drawing. Perception, 1, 123–40.
Freeman, N. H. (1972). Process and product in children's drawing. Perception, 1, 123–40.
Freeman, N. H. (1975). Do children draw men with arms coming out of the head? Nature, 254, 416–17.
Freeman, N. H. (1980). Strategies of representation in young children. London: Academic Press.
Freeman, N. H. (1980). Strategies of representation in young children. London: Academic Press.
Freeman, N. H. (1980). Strategies of representation in young children. London: Academic Press.
Freeman, N. H. (1980). Strategies of representation in young children. London: Academic Press.
Freeman, N. H. (1980). Strategies of representation in young children: analysis of spatial skills and drawing processes. London: Academic Press.
Freeman, N. H. (1980). Strategies of representation. New York: Academic Press.
Freeman, N. H. (1986). How should a cube be drawn? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 317–22.
Freeman, N. H. (1986). How should a cube be drawn? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 317–22.
Freeman, N. H. (1993). Drawing: public instrument of representation. In C. Pratt and A. F. Garton (eds.), Systems of representation in children (pp. 113–32). Chichester: Wiley.
Freeman, N. H. (1995). The emergence of a framework theory of pictorial reasoning. In C. Lange-Küttner and G. V. Thomas (eds.), Drawing and looking: theoretical approaches to pictorial representation in children (pp. 135–46). London: Pearson/Prentice Hall (distributed by www.amazon.co.uk).
Freeman, N. H. (1995). The emergence of a framework theory of pictorial reasoning. In C. Lange-Küttner and G. V. Thomas (eds.), Drawing and looking: theoretical approaches to pictorial representation in children (pp. 135–46). London: Pearson/Prentice Hall (distributed by www.amazon.co.uk).
Freeman, N. H. (2000). Communication and representation: why mentalistic reasoning is a lifelong endeavour. In P. Mitchell and K. J. Riggs (eds.), Children's reasoning and the mind (pp. 349–67). Hove: Psychology Press.
Freeman, N. H. (2004). Aesthetic judgement and reasoning. In E. W. Eisner and M. D. Day (eds.), Handbook of research and policy in art education (pp. 359–78). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum/National Art Education Association (USA).
Freeman, N. H. and Cox, M. V. (eds.) (1985). Visual order. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Freeman, N. H. and Cox, M. V. (eds.) (1985). Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Freeman, N. H. and Hargreaves, S. (1977). Directed movements and the body-proportion effect in preschool children's human figure drawings. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 28, 227–35.
Freeman, N. H. and Janikoun, R. (1972). Intellectual realism in children's drawings of a familiar object with distinctive features. Child Development, 43, 1116–21.
Freeman, N. H., Eiser, C. and Sayers, J. (1977). Children's strategies in producing three-dimensional relationships on a two-dimensional surface. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 23, 305–14.
Freeman, N. H., Hood, B. M. and Meehan, C. (2004). Young children who abandon error behaviourally still have to free themselves mentally: a retrospective test for change in intuitive physics. Developmental Science, 7, 277–82.
Frith, U. (2003). Autism: explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell.
Frith, U. (2003). Autism: explaining the enigma. Second edition. Oxford: Blackwell.
Gallo, F., Golomb, C. and Barroso, A. (2003). Compositional strategies in drawing: the effects of two- and three-dimensional media. Visual Arts Research, 29, 2–23.
Gallup, G. G., Jr (1970). Chimpanzees: self-recognition. Science, 167, 86–7.
Gallup, G. G., Jr (1970). Chimpanzees: self-recognition. Science, 167, 86–7.
Gallup, G. G., Jr (1977). Self-recognition in primates: a comparative approach to the bidirectional properties of consciousness. American Psychologist, 32, 329–38.
Gallup, G. G., Jr (1982). Self-awareness and the emergence of mind in primates. American Journal of Primatology, 2, 237–48.
Gardner, H. (1980). Artful scribbles. New York: Basic Books.
Gellert, E. (1968). Comparison of children's self-drawing with their drawings of other persons. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 26, 123–38.
Gellert, E. (1968). Comparison of children's self-drawing with their drawings of other persons. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 26, 123–38.
Gelman, S. A. and Ebeling, K. S. (1998). Shape and representational status in children's early naming. Cognition, 66, B35–B47.
Gergely, G. (1994). From self-recognition to theory of mind. In S. T. Parker, R. W. Mitchell and M. L. Boccia (eds.), Self-awareness in animals and humans: developmental perspectives (pp. 51–60). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Gergely, G. (2006). The development of understanding self and agency. In U. Goswami (ed.), Childhood cognitive development (pp. 26–46). Oxford: Blackwell.
Germond, P. and Livet, J. (2001). An Egyptian bestiary. London: Thames and Hudson.
Gesell, A. and Ames, L. B. (1946). The development of directionality in drawing. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 68, 45–61.
Gibson, J. J. (1950). The perception of the visual world. Boston: Riverside Press.
Gibson, J. J. (1962). Observations on active touch. Psychological Review, 69, 447–9.
Gibson, K. R. (2007). Epigenesis, mental construction, and the emergence of language and toolmaking. In D. A. Washburn (ed.), Primate perspectives on behaviour and cognition (pp. 269–78). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Glenn, S. M., Bradshaw, K. and Sharp, M. (1995). Handedness and the direction and sequencing in children's drawings of people. Educational Psychology, 15, 11–21.
Golomb, C. (1973). Children's representation of the human figure: the effects of models, media and instruction. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 87, 197–251.
Golomb, C. (1974). Young children's sculpture and drawing. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Golomb, C. (1987). The development of compositional strategies in children's drawings. Visual Arts Research, 13, 42–52.
Golomb, C. (1992). The child's creation of a pictorial world. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Golomb, C. (1992). The child's creation of a pictorial world. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Golomb, C. (2002). Child art in context: a cultural and comparative perspective. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Golomb, C. (2002). Child arts in context: a cultural and comparative perspective. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Gombrich, E. H. (1960). Art and Illusion: a study in the psychology of pictorial representation. London: Pantheon Books.
Gombrich, E. H. (1984). Art and illusion. (Bollingen Series 35.) Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Goodenough, F. L. (1926). Measurement of intelligence by drawings. New York: World Book Company.
Goodenough, F. L. (1926). Measurement of intelligence by drawings. New York: World Book Company.
Goodenough, F. L. and Harris, D. B. (1950). Studies in the psychology of children's drawings. Ⅱ. Psychological Bulletin, 47, 369–433.
Goodenough, F. L. and Harris, D. B. (1950). Studies in the psychology of children's drawings. Ⅱ. Psychological Bulletin, 47, 369–433.
Goodman, N. (1968). Languages of art. Indianapolis: Bobbs-Merrill.
Goodnow, J. (1977). Children drawing. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Goodnow, J. (1977). Children drawing. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Goodnow, J. (1977). Children's drawing. London: Fontana/Open Books.
Goodnow, J. (1978). Visible thinking: cognitive aspects of change in drawings. Child Development, 49, 637–41.
Goodnow, J. and Levine, R. A. (1973). The grammar of action: sequence and syntax in children's copying behaviour. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 82–98.
Goodnow, J. J. (1977). Children's drawing. London: Open Books.
Goodnow, J. J. (1977). Children's drawings. London: Fontana.
Goodnow, J. J. (1977). Children's drawings. London: Fontana.
Goodnow, J. J. (1977). Children's drawings. London: Fontana.
Goodnow, J. J. and Levine, R. A. (1973). ‘The grammar of action': sequence and syntax in children's copying. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 82–98.
Goodnow, J. J. and Levine, R. A. (1973). The ‘grammar of action’: sequence and syntax in children's copying behaviour. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 82–98.
Goodnow, J. J., Friedman, S. L., Bernbaum, M. and Lehman, E. B. (1973). Direction and sequence in copying: the effect of learning to write in English and Hebrew. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 4, 263–82.
Gopnik, A. and Astington, J. W. (1988). Children's understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance reality distinction. Child Development, 59, 26–37.
Gopnik, A. and Rosati, A. (2001). Duck or rabbit? Reversing ambiguous figures and understanding ambiguous representations. Developmental Science, 4, 175–83.
Goswami, U. (1992). Analogical reasoning in children. Hove: Erlbaum.
Gottlieb, G. (2007). Probabilistic epigenesis. Developmental Science, 10, 1–11.
Graff, J. (2006). Saving beauty. Mold and bureaucracy threaten France's Lascaux cave. Time, 167, 36–42.
Greenberg, J. and Musham, C. (1981). Avoiding and seeking self-focused attention. Journal of Research in Personality, 15, 191–200.
Greer, T. and Lockman, J. J. (1998). Using writing instruments: invariances in young children and adults. Child Development, 69, 888–902.
Gross, J. and Hayne, H. (1999). Young children's recognition and description of their own and others’ drawings. Developmental Science, 2, 476–89.
Gross, J. and Hayne, H. (1999). Young children's recognition and description of their own and others' drawings. Developmental Science, 2, 476–89.
Gross, J. and Hayne, H. (1999). Young children's recognition and description of their own and others' drawings. Developmental Science, 2, 476–89.
Hagen, M. A. (1985). There is no development in art. In N. Freeman and M. V. Cox (eds.), Visual order (pp. 78–100). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Halligan, P. W. and Marshall, J. C. (1998). Neglect of awareness. Consciousness and Cognition, 7, 356–80.
Halverson, H. M. (1931). An experimental study of prehension in infants by means of systematic cinema records. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 10, 107–286.
Harel, J., Eshel, Y., Ganor, O. and Scher, A. (2002). Antecedents of mirror self-recognition of toddlers: emotional availability, birth order and gender. Infant Mental Health Journal, 23, 293–309.
Hargreaves, D. J., Jones, P. M. and Martin, D. (1981). The air gap phenomenon in children's landscape drawings. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 32, 11–20.
Harris, D. B. (1963). Children's drawings as measures of intellectual maturity. New York: Harper and Row.
Hartley, J. L., Somerville, S. C., Von Cziesch Jensen, D. and Eliefa, C. C. (1982). Abstraction of individual styles from the drawings of five-year-old children. Child Development, 53, 1193–1214.
Hauser, M. D., Miller, C. T., Liu, K. and Gupta, R. (2001). Cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) fail to show mirror-guided self-exploration. American Journal of Primatology, 53, 131–7.
Hayes, B. K. and Hennessy, R. (1996). The nature and development of nonverbal implicit memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 22–43.
Hayworth, K. J. and Biederman, I. (2006). Neural evidence for intermediate representations in object recognition. Vision Research, 46, 4024–31.
Hegel, G. W. F. (1812/1975). Wissenschaft der Logik. Erster Band: Die objective Logik [Science of logic. First volume: The objective logic]. Hamburg: Felix Meiner [reprint of the first edition by Schrag, Nuremberg].
Heller, M. A. and Kennedy, J. M. (1990). Perspective-taking, pictures and the blind. Perception and Psychophysics, 48, 459–66.
Heller, M. A., Brackett, D. E., Scroggs, E., Steffen, H., Heatherly, K. and Salik, S. (2002). Tangible pictures: viewpoint effects and linear perspective in visually impaired people. Perception, 31, 747–69.
Heller, M. A., Kennedy, J. M., Clark, A., McCarthy, M., Borgert, A., Wemple, L., Fulkerson, E., Kaffel, N., Duncan, A. and Riddle, T. (2006). Viewpoint and orientation influence picture recognition in the blind. Perception, 35, 1397–1420.
Hesselberg, T. and Vollrath, F. (2004). The effects of neurotoxins on web-geometry and web-building behaviour in Araneus diadematus Cl. Physiology and Behaviour, 82, 519–29.
Hildebrand, D. K., Laing, J. D. and Rosenthal, H. (1977). Prediction analysis of cross classifications. New York: Wiley.
Hill, A. L. (1977). Idiot savants: rate of incidence. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 44, 161–2.
Hochberg, J. and Brooks, V. (1962). Pictorial recognition as an unlearned ability. American Journal of Psychology, 75, 624–8.
Hodges, J. R., Graham, N. and Patterson, K. (1995). Charting the progression in semantic dementia: implications for the organization of semantic memory. Memory, 3, 463–95.
Hodges, J. R., Patterson, K. and Tyler, L. K. (1994). Loss of semantic memory: implications for the modularity of mind. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 11, 505–42.
Hodges, J. R., Patterson, K., Oxbury, S. and Funnell, E. (1992). Semantic dementia: progressive fluent aphasia with temporal lobe atrophy. Brain, 115, 1783–1806.
Hopkins, R. (2003). Touching, seeing and appreciating pictures. In E. Axel and N. Levent (eds.), Art beyond sight: a resource guide to art, creativity and visual impairment (pp. 186–99). New York: AFB Press.
Howard, D. and Patterson, K. (1992). Pyramids and palmtrees: a test of semantic access from words and pictures. Bury St Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company.
Huang-Pollock, C. L., Carr, T. H. and Nigg, J. T. (2004). Perceptual load influences early versus late attentional selection in children and adults. Developmental Psychology, 40, 545–58.
Humphrey, N. K. (1976). The social function of intellect. In P. P. Bates and R. A. Hinde (eds.), Growing points in ethology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Humphreys, G. W. and Riddoch, M. J. (1993). Interactions between object and space systems revealed through neuropsychology. In D. E. Meyer and S. Kornblum (eds.), Attention and performance ⅩⅣ: synergies in experimental psychology, artificial intelligence, and cognitive neuroscience (pp. 143–75). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Hund, A. M. and Plumert, J. M. (2002). Delay-induced bias in children's memory for location. Child Development, 73, 829–40.
Hund, A. M. and Plumert, J. M. (2005). The stability and flexibility of spatial categories. Cognitive Psychology, 50, 1–44.
Hyatt, C. W. and Hopkins, W. D. (1994). Self-awareness in bonobos and chimpanzees: a comparative perspective. In S. Parker, R. W. Mitchell and M. Boccia (eds.), Self-awareness in animals and humans: developmental perspectives (pp. 248–53). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Ibbotson, A. and Bryant, P. E. (1978). The perpendicular error and the vertical effect. Perception, 5, 319–26.
Ingram, N. (1985). Three into two won't go: symbolic and spatial coding processes in young children's drawings. In N. H. Freeman and M. V. Cox (eds.), Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation (pp. 231–47). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Ingram, N. (1985). Three into two won't go: symbolic and spatial coding processes in young children's drawings. In N. H. Freeman and M. V. Cox (eds.), Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation (pp. 231–47). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Ino, T., Asada, T., Ito, J., Kimura, T. and Fukuyama, H. (2003). Parieto-frontal networks for clock drawing revealed with fMRI. Neuroscience Research, 45, 71–7.
Inoue-Nakamura, N. (2001). Mirror self-recognition in primates: an ontogenetic and a phylogenetic approach. In T. Matsuzawa (ed.), Primate origins of human cognition and behaviour (pp. 297–312). New York: Springer.
Itakura, S. (2001). The level of self-knowledge in nonhuman primates: from the perspective of comparative cognitive science. In T. Matsuzawa (ed.), Primate origins of human cognition and behaviour (pp. 313–29). New York: Springer.
Jablonka, E. and Lamb, M. (2007). Evolution in four dimensions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 353–65.
Jansson, G. (2001). The potential importance of perceptual filling-in for haptic perception of virtual object form. In C. Baber, M. Faint, S. Wall and A. M. Wing (eds.), Eurohaptics 2001. Conference Proceedings. Educational Technology Research Papers (Vol. 12, pp. 72–5). Birmingham: Birmingham University Press.
Jastrzembski, T. S., Charness, N. and Vasyukova, C. (2006). Expertise and age effects on knowledge activation in chess. Psychology and Aging, 21, 401–5.
Jefferies, E., Jones, R. W., Bateman, D. and Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2005). A semantic contribution to nonword recall? Evidence for intact phonological processes in semantic dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22, 183–212.
Jenkins, L., Myerson, J., Joerding, J. A. and Hale, S. (2000). Converging evidence that visuospatial cognition is more age-sensitive than verbal cognition. Psychology and Aging, 15, 157–75.
Johnson, D. B. (1982). Self-recognition in infants. Infant Behaviour and Development, 6, 211–22.
Johnson, M. H. and Morton, J. (1991). Biology and cognitive development: the case of face recognition. Oxford: Blackwell.
Jolley, R. P. (in press). Children and pictures: drawing and understanding. Oxford: Blackwell.
Jolly, A. (1966). Lemur social behaviour and primate intelligence. Science, 153, 501–6.
Juricevic, I. and Kennedy, J. M. (2006). Looking at perspective pictures from too far, too close and just right. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 448–61.
Kail, R. (1986). Sources of age differences in speed of processing. Child Development, 57, 969–87.
Kanizsa, G. (1979). Organization in vision: essays on Gestalt perception. New York: Praeger.
Kant, I. (1781/1980). Kritik der reinen Vernunft [Critique of pure reason]. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp.
Kappers, A. M. L. and Koenderink, J. J. (2002). Continuum of haptic space. In L. Albertazzi (ed.), Unfolding perceptual continua (pp. 29–79). Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1990). Constraints on representational change: evidence from children's drawing. Cognition, 34, 57–83.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1990). Constraints on representational change: evidence from children's drawing. Cognition, 34, 57–83.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1990). Constraints on representational change: evidence from children's drawing. Cognition, 34, 57–83.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1990). Constraints on representational change: evidence from children's drawings. Cognition, 34, 57–83.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1990). Constraints on representational change: evidence from children's drawings. Cognition, 34, 57–83.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: a developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: a developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1995). Beyond modularity: a developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1999). Taking development seriously. Human Development, 42, 325–7.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2007). Atypical epigenesis. Developmental Science, 10, 84–8.
Keller, H., Kartner, J., Borke, J., Yovsi, R. and Kleis, A. (2005). Parenting styles and the development of the categorical self: a longitudinal study on mirror self-recognition in Cameroonian Nso and German families. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29, 496–504.
Keller, O. (1909). Die antike Tierwelt, I [The antique animal world, I]. Leipzig: Engelmann.
Kellogg, R. (1969). Analyzing children's art. Palo Alto, CA: Mayfield.
Kellogg, R. (1969). Analyzing children's art. Palo Alto, CA: National Press Books.
Kellogg, R. (1970). Analysing children's art. Palo Alto, CA: National Press Books.
Kemler Nelson, D. G. (1984). The effect of intention on what concepts are acquired. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 23, 734–59.
Kemler Nelson, D. G. (1989). The nature and occurrence of holistic processing. In B. E. Shepp and S. Ballesteros (eds.), Object perception (pp. 357–86). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kennedy, J. M. (1993). Drawing and the blind. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Kennedy, J. M. (2003). Drawings from Gaia, a blind girl. Perception, 32, 321–40.
Kennedy, J. M. (2006). Blind man draws using diminution in three dimensions. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 3, 506–9.
Kennedy, J. M. (2006). Form, projection and pictures for the blind. In M. A. Heller and S. Ballesteros (eds.), Haptics, blindness and neuroscience (pp. 73–93). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kennedy, J. M. and Juricevic, I. (2003). Haptics and projection: drawings by Tracy, a blind adult. Perception, 32, 1059–71.
Kennedy, J. M. and Merkas, C. E. (2000). Depictions of motion devised by a blind person. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 7, 700–6.
Kerschensteiner, D. G. (1905). Die Entwickelung der zeichnerischen Begabung. Munich: Gerber.
Kerschensteiner, G. (1905). Die Entwickelung der zeichnerischen Begabung. Munich: Carl Gerber.
Kerschensteiner, G. (1905). Die Entwicklung der zeichnerischen Begabung. Munich: Gerber.
Kho, K. H., Van den Bergh, W. M., Spetgensx, W. P. J. and Leijten, F. S. S. (2006). Figuring out drawing-induced epilepsy. Neurology, 66, 723–6.
Kim, K. H. (2006). Can we trust creativity tests? A review of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking (TTCT). Creative Research Journal, 8, 3–14.
Kinsbourne, M. (1993). Orientational bias model of unilateral neglect: evidence from attentional gradients within hemispace. In I. H. Robertson and J. C. Marshall (eds.), Unilateral neglect: clinical and experimental studies (pp. 63–86). Hove: Erlbaum.
Kliegl, R., Smith, J. and Baltes, P. B. (1986). Testing-the-limits, expertise, and memory in adulthood and old age. In F. Klix and H. Hagendorf (eds.), Human memory and cognitive capabilities (pp. 395–407). Amsterdam: North-Holland/Elsevier.
Kliegl, R., Smith, J. and Baltes, P. B. (1989). Testing-the-limits and the study of adult age differences in cognitive plasticity of a mnemonic skill. Developmental Psychology, 25, 247–56.
Kliegl, R., Smith, J. and Baltes, P. B. (1990). On the locus and process of magnification of age differences during mnemonic training. Developmental Psychology, 26, 894–904.
Kosslyn, S. M. (1994). Image and brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kosslyn, S. M., Heldmeyer, K. H. and Locklear, E. P. (1977). Children's drawings as data about internal representations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 23, 191–211.
Krampe, R. T. (2002). Aging, expertise, and fine motor movement. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 769–76.
Krampe, R. T. and Baltes, P. B. (2003). Intelligence as adaptive resource development and resource allocation: a new look through the lenses of SOC and expertise. In R. J. Sternberg and E. L. Grigorenko (eds.), Perspectives on the psychology of abilities, competencies, and expertise (pp. 31–69). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Krampe, R. T. and Ericsson, K. A. (1996). Maintaining excellence: deliberate practice and elite performance in young and older pianists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 331–59.
Krampe, R. T., Engbert, R. and Kliegl, R. (2001). The effects of expertise and age on rhythm production: adaptations to timing and sequencing constraints. Brain and Cognition, 48, 179–94.
Kubovy, M. (1986). The psychology of perspective and Renaissance art. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Kuhn, D. and Pease, M. (2006). Do children and adults learn differently? Journal of Cognition and Development, 7, 279–94.
Lambon Ralph, M. A. and Howard, D. (2000). Gogi aphasia or semantic dementia? Simulating and assessing poor verbal comprehension in a case of progressive fluent aphasia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 437–65.
Lambon Ralph, M. A., Graham, K. S., Patterson, K. and Hodges, J. R. (1999). Is a picture worth a thousand words? Evidence from concept definitions by patients with semantic dementia. Brain and Language, 70, 309–35.
Landerer, C. (2000). Kunstgeschichte als Kognitiongeschichte: ein Beitrag zur genetischen Kulturpsychologie [History of art as history of cognition: a contribution to genetic culture psychology]. Doctoral dissertation, University of Salzburg.
Lange-Küttner, C. (1989). Raumbegriff und Objektbeziehungen beim Kind [Space concept and object relations in the child]. Frankfurt am Main: Lang.
Lange-Küttner, C. (1994). Gestalt und Konstruktion: die Entwicklung der grafischen Kompetenz beim Kind [Gestalt and construction: the development of graphic competence in the child]. Berne: Huber.
Lange-Küttner, C. (1997). Development of size modification of human figure drawings in spatial axes systems of varying complexity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 66, 264–78.
Lange-Küttner, C. (1997). Development of size modification of human figure drawings in spatial axes systems of varying complexity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 66, 264–78.
Lange-Küttner, C. (1997). Development of size modification of human figure drawings in spatial axes systems of varying complexity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 66, 264–78.
Lange-Küttner, C. (1998). Pressure, velocity and time in speeded drawing of basic graphic pattern by young children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 86, 1299–1310.
Lange-Küttner, C. (1998). Pressure, velocity and time in speeded drawing of basic graphic pattern by young children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 86, 1299–1310.
Lange-Küttner, C. (2000). The role of object violation in the development of visual analysis. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 86, 1299–1310.
Lange-Küttner, C. (2000). The role of object violations in the development of visual analysis. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 90, 3–24.
Lange-Küttner, C. (2000). The role of object violations in the development of visual analysis. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 90, 3–24.
Lange-Küttner, C. (2004). More evidence on size modification in spatial axes systems of varying complexity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 88, 171–92.
Lange-Küttner, C. (2004). More evidence on size modification in spatial axes systems of varying complexity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 88, 171–92.
Lange-Küttner, C. (2004). More evidence on size modification in spatial axes systems of varying complexity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 88, 171–92.
Lange-Küttner, C. (2005). The logic of spatial axes systems. Paper presented at the 12th European Conference on Developmental Psychology. Tenerife, 24–28 August.
Lange-Küttner, C. (2006). Drawing boundaries: from individual to common region – the development of spatial region attribution in children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 419–27.
Lange-Küttner, C. (2008). Size and contour as crucial parameters in children drawing images. In C. Milbrath and H. M. Trautner (eds.), Children's understanding and production of pictures, drawing, and art: theoretical and empirical approaches (pp. 89–106). Göttingen: Hogrefe.
Lange-Küttner, C. (under revision). Habitual size and projective size: the logic of spatial systems in children's drawings.
Lange-Küttner, C. and Crichton, M. T. (1999). Change of spatial field effects in 16- to 20-week-old infants. Brain and Cognition, 39, 75–92.
Lange-Küttner, C. and Green, H. (2007). What is the age of mental rotation? Proceedings of the 6th IEEE International Conference on Development and Learning, pp. 259–63.
Lange-Küttner, C. and Reith, E. (1995). The transformation of figurative thought: implications of Piaget and Inhelder's developmental theory for children's drawings. In C. Lange-Küttner and G. V. Thomas (eds.), Drawing and looking (pp. 75–92). London: Pearson/Prentice Hall (distributed by www.amazon.co.uk).
Lange-Küttner, C. and Thomas, G. V. (1995). Drawing and looking: theoretical approaches to pictorial representation in children. London: Pearson/Prentice Hall (distributed by www.amazon.co.uk).
Lange-Küttner, C. and Thomas, G. V. (eds.) (1995). Drawing and looking. London: Pearson/Prentice Hall (distributed by www.amazon.co.uk).
Lange-Küttner, C., Kerzmann, A. and Heckhausen, J. (2002). The emergence of visually realistic contour in the drawing of the human figure. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 439–63.
Lange-Küttner, C., Kerzmann, A. and Heckhausen, J. (2002). The emergence of visually realistic contour in the drawing of the human figure. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3, 439–63.
Laursen, B. (2006). Tegning, cognition og innovation – om komplekse tegne-kompetencer [Drawing, cognition and innovation – on complex drawing abilities]. Billedpaedagogisk Tidskrift, 2, 8–10.
Leavens, D. A., Hopkins, W. D. and Bard, K. A. (2005). Understanding the point of chimpanzee pointing: epigenesis and ecological validity. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 185–9.
Leavens, D. A., Hopkins, W. D. and Bard, K. A. (in press). The heterochronic origins of explicit reference. In J. Zlatev, T. P. Racine, C. Sinha and E. Itkonen (eds.), The shared mind: perspectives on intersubjectivity. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Lederman, S. J., Klatzky, R. L., Chataway, C. and Summers, C. (1990). Visual mediation and the haptic recognition of two-dimensional pictures of common objects. Perception and Psychophysics, 47, 54–64.
Lee, M. (1989). When is an object not an object? The effect of ‘meaning’ upon the copying of line drawings. British Journal of Psychology, 80, 15–37.
Lee, M. and Bremner, J. G. (1987). The representation of depth in children's drawings of a table. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39A, 479–96.
Lee, M. and Bremner, J. G. (1987). The representation of depth in children's drawings of a table. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39A, 479–96.
Leekam, S. R. and Perner, J. (1991). Does the autistic child have a metarepresentational deficit? Cognition, 40, 203–18.
Leroi-Gourhan, A. (1982). The dawn of European art. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: the origins of ‘Theory of Mind’. Psychological Review, 94, 412–26.
Leslie, A. M. and Polizzi, P. (1998). Inhibitory processing in the false belief task: two conjectures. Developmental Science, 1, 247–58.
Leslie, A. M. and Thaiss, L. (1992). Domain specificity in conceptual development: neuropsychological evidence from autism. Cognition, 43, 225–51.
Lewin, K. (1951). Field theory in social science. New York: Harper Brothers.
Lewin, K. (1963/1982). Feldtheorie: Kurt-Lewin-Werkausgabe, Band 4 [Field theory. Kurt Lewin Collected Writings, Vol. 4]. Berne and Stuttgart: Huber and Klett-Cotta.
Lewis, C., Russell, C. and Berridge, D. (1993). When is a mug not a mug? Effects of content, naming, and instructions on children's drawings. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 291–302.
Lewis, C., Russell, C. and Berridge, D. (1993). When is a mug not a mug? Effects of content, naming, and instructions on children's drawings. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 291–302.
Lewis, M. and Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Social cognition and the acquisition of self. New York: Plenum Press.
Lewis, M., Brooks Gunn, J. and Jaskir, J. (1985). Individual differences in visual self-recognition as a function of mother–infant attachment relationship. Developmental Psychology, 21, 1181–7.
Lewis, M., Sullivan, M. W., Stanger, C. and Weiss, M. (1989). Self development and self-conscious emotions. Child Development, 60, 146–56.
Li, S.-C. (2003). Biocultural orchestration of developmental plasticity across levels: the interplay of biology and culture in shaping the mind and behaviour across the lifespan. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 171–94.
Li, S.-C. and Lindenberger, U. (2002). Co-constructed functionality instead of functional normality. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 761–2.
Li, S.-C., Lindenberger, U., Hommel, B., Aschersleben, G., Prinz, W. and Baltes, P. B. (2004). Transformations in the couplings among intellectual abilities and constituent cognitive processes across the life span. Psychological Science, 15, 155–63.
Liben, L. S. (1988). Conceptual issues in the development of spatial cognition. In J. Stiles-Davis, M. Kritchevsky and U. Bellugi (eds.), Spatial cognition: brain bases and development (pp. 167–94). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lieblich, A., Ninio, A. and Kugelmass, S. (1975). Developmental trends in directionality of drawing in Jewish and Arab Israeli children. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 6, 504–11.
Light, P. (1985). The development of view-specific representation when considered from a socio-cognitive standpoint. In N. H. Freeman and M. V. Cox (eds.), Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation (pp. 214–30). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Light, P. H. and Humphreys, J. (1981). Internal spatial relationships in young children's drawings. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 31, 521–30.
Light, P. H. and MacIntosh, E. (1980). Depth relationships in young children's drawings. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 30, 79–87.
Lin, A. C., Bard, K. A. and Anderson, J. R. (1992). Development of self-recognition in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 106, 120–7.
Lindenberger, U. and Baltes, P. B. (1995). Testing-the-limits and experimental simulation: two methods to explicate the role of learning in development. Journal of Human Development, 38, 349–60.
Lindenberger, U., Kliegl, R. and Baltes, P. B. (1992). Professional expertise does not eliminate age differences in imagery-based memory performance during adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 7, 585–93.
Lindenberger, U., Li, S.-C. and Bäckman, L. (2006). Delineating brain-behaviour mappings across the lifespan: substantive and methodological advances in developmental neuroscience. Editorial. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 713–17.
Logan, G. D. (1996). The CODE theory of visual attention. An integration of space-based and object-based attention. Psychological Review, 103, 603–49.
Logie, R. H. (1986). Visuo-spatial processing in working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38A, 229–47.
Longcamp, M., Zerbato-Poudou, M.-T. and Velay, J.-L. (2005). The influence of writing practice on letter recognition in preschool children: a comparison between handwriting and typing. Acta Psychologica, 119, 67–79.
Lopes, D. (1997). Understanding pictures. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Lopes, D. M. M. (2003). Are pictures visual: a brief history of an idea. In E. Axel and N. Levent (eds.), Art beyond sight: a resource guide to art, creativity and visual impairment (pp. 176–85). New York: AFB Press.
Lowery, N., Giovanni, L., Harper Mozley, L., Arnold, S. E., Bilker, W. B., Gur, R. E. and Moberg, P. J. (2003). Relationship between clock-drawing and neuropsychological and functional status in elderly institutionalized patients with schizophrenia. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 11, 621–8.
Lukens, H. T. (1986). A study of children's drawings in the early years. Pedagogical Seminary, 4, 79–110.
Luquet, G. (1913). Les dessins d'un enfant [The drawings of a child]. Paris: Alcan.
Luquet, G. H. (1927). Le dessin enfantin [Children's drawing]. Paris: Alcan.
Luquet, G. H. (1927). Le dessin enfantin [Children's drawing]. Paris: Alcan.
Luquet, G. H. (1927). Le dessin enfantin [Children's drawing]. Paris: Alcan.
Luquet, G. H. (1927). Le dessin enfantin [Children's drawings]. Paris: Alcan.
Luquet, G. H. (2001). Children's drawings. Trans. Alan Costall. London: Free Association Books.
Luquet, G.-H. (1927). Le dessin enfantin [Children's drawing]. Paris: Alcan.
Maguire, E. A., Gadian, D. G., Johnsrude, I. S., Good, C. D., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., et al. (2000). Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 97, 4398–4403.
Makuuchi, M., Kaminaga, T. and Sugishita, M. (2003). Both parietal lobes are involved in drawing: a functional MRI study and implications for constructional apraxia. Cognitive Brain Research, 16, 338–47.
Mandler, J. M. (1988). The development of spatial cognition: on topological and euclidian representation. In J. Stiles-Davis, M. Kritchevsky and U. Bellugi (eds.), Spatial cognition: brain bases and development (pp. 423–32). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Manly, T., Dobler, V. B., Dodds, C. M. and George, M. A. (2005). Rightward shift in spatial awareness with declining alertness. Neuropsychologia, 43, 1721–8.
Marot, V. and Vinter, A. (2003). Rôle de la complexité et de l'orientation d'une figure géométrique sur l'application des règles de production graphique chez l'enfant et l'adulte [The role of complexity and orientation of a geometric figure for the application of graphic production rules in the child and adult]. Archives de Psychologie, 70, 175–92.
Marr, D. (1982). Vision. San Francisco, CA: Freeman.
Marteniuk, R. G., McKenzie, C. L., Jeannerod, M., Athenes, S. and Dugas, C. (1987). Constraints on human arm movement trajectories. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 41, 365–78.
Martlew, M. (1992). Pen grips: their relationship to letter/word formation and literacy knowledge in children starting school. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 23, 165–85.
Masunaga, H. and Horn, J. (2001). Expertise in relation to aging changes in components of intelligence. Psychology and Aging, 16, 293–311.
Matsuzawa, T. (2007). Comparative cognitive development. Developmental Science, 10, 97–103.
Matthews, J. (2004). The art of infancy. In E. W. Eisner and M. D. Day (eds.), Handbook of research and policy in art education (pp. 253–98). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Mayer-Hillebrand, F. (1947). Die Perspektive in psychologischer Betrachtung. Wiener Zeitschrift für Philosophie, Psychologie und Pädagogik, 1, 141–61.
Maylor, E. A. (1994). Ageing and retrieval of specialized and general knowledge: performance of “Masterminds”. British Journal of Psychology, 85, 105–14.
Mecklinger, A. and Meinshausen, R.-M. (1998). Recognition memory for object form and object location: an event-related potential study. Memory and Cognition, 26, 1068–88.
Menzel, E., Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. and Lawson, J. (1985). Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) spatial problem solving with the use of mirrors and televised equivalents of mirrors. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 99, 211–17.
Meulenbroek, R. G. J., Vinter, A. and Mounoud, P. (1993). Development of start rotation principle in circle production. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11, 307–20.
Michałowski, K. (1969). Art of ancient Egypt. London: Thames and Hudson.
Milbrath, C. (1998). Patterns of artistic development in children. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Milbrath, C. (1998). Patterns of artistic development in children: comparative studies of talent. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Miles, H. L. W. (1994). Me Chantek: the development of self-awareness in a signing orangutan. In S. T. Parker, R. W. Mitchell and M. Boccia (eds.), Self-awareness in animals and humans: developmental perspectives (pp. 254–72). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Millar, S. (2006). Processing spatial information from touch and movement: implications from and for neuroscience. In M. A. Heller and S. Ballesteros (eds.), Haptics, blindness and neuroscience (pp. 25–48). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Miller, B. L. (2006). Figuring out drawing-induced epilepsy. Commentary. Neurology, 66, 619.
Mitchell, P. and Isaacs, J. E. (1994). Understanding of verbal representation in children with autism: the case of referential opacity. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 439–54.
Mitchell, P., Ropar, D., Ackroyd, K. and Rajendran, G. (2005). How perception impacts on drawings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 996–1003.
Mitchell, P., Ropar, D., Ackroyd, K. and Rajendran, G. (2005). How perception impacts on drawings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 996–1003.
Mitchell, R. (1993). Mental models of mirror-self-recognition: two theories. New Ideas in Psychology, 11, 295–325.
Moore, V. (1987). The influence of experience on children's drawings of a familiar and unfamiliar object. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 221–9.
Morassi, L. and Tallandini, M. A. (2006). Graphic representation: a model of interaction between the spatial and the symbolic component. Paper presented at the XIVth Symposium of Perception and Cognition. Trieste, Italy.
Moritz, C. H., Johnson, S. C., McMillan, K. M., Haughton, V. M. and Meyerand, M. E. (2004). Functional MRI neuroanatomic correlates of the Hooper Visual Organization Test. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 939–47.
Morra, S. (1994). Issues in working memory measurement: testing for M capacity. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 17, 143–59.
Morra, S. (1995). A neo-Piagetian approach to children's drawings. In C. Lange-Küttner and G. V. Thomas (eds.), Drawing and looking: theoretical approaches to pictorial representation in children (pp. 93–106). London: Pearson/Prentice Hall (distributed by www.amazon.co.uk).
Morra, S. (2000). A new model of verbal short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 75, 191–227.
Morra, S. (2002). On the relationship between partial occlusion drawing, M capacity, and field independence. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 421–38.
Morra, S. (2005). Cognitive aspects of change in drawings: a neo-Piagetian theoretical account. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 317–41.
Morra, S. (2008). Memory components and control processes in children's drawing. In C. Milbrath and H. M. Trautner (eds.), Children's understanding and production of pictures, drawing, and art: theoretical and empirical approaches (pp. 53–85). Göttingen: Hogrefe.
Morra, S. (in press). A test of a neo-Piagetian model of the Water Level Task. European Journal of Developmental Psychology.
Morra, S., Angi, A. and Tomat, L. (1996). Planning, encoding, and overcoming conflict in partial occlusion drawing: a neo-Piagetian model and an experimental analysis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 61, 276–301.
Morra, S., Caloni, B. and d'Amico, M. R. (1994). Working memory and the intentional depiction of emotions. Archives de Psychologie, 62, 71–87.
Morra, S., Gobbo, C., Marini, Z. and Sheese, R. (2008). Cognitive development: a neo-Piagetian perspective. New York: Erlbaum.
Morra, S., Moizo, C. and Scopesi, A. (1988). Working memory (or the M operator) and the planning of children's drawings. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 46, 41–73.
Morra, S., Moizo, C. and Scopesi, A. (1988). Working memory (or the M operator) and the planning of children's drawings. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 41–73.
Morrow, D. G. and Leirer, V. O. (1997). Aging, pilot performance, and expertise. In A. D. Fisk and W. A. Rogers (eds.), Handbook of human factors and the older adult (pp. 199–230). New York: Academic Press.
Morrow, D. G., Ridolfo, H. E., Menard, W. E., Sanborn, W. E., Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., Magnor, C. et al. (2003). Environmental support promotes expertise-based mitigation of age differences in pilot communication tasks. Psychology and Aging, 18, 268–84.
Mottron, L., Belleville, S. and Ménard, E. (1999). Local bias in autistic subjects as evidenced by graphic tasks: perceptual hierarchization or working memory deficit? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 743–55.
Mounoud, P. (1988). The ontogenesis of different types of thought: language and motor behaviours as non-specific manifestations. In L. Weiskrantz (ed.), Thought without language (pp. 25–45). Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Mounoud, P. (1996). A recursive transformation of central cognitive mechanisms: the shift from partial to whole representation. In P. Sameroff and M. Haith (eds.), Reason and responsibility: the passage through childhood (pp. 85–110). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: the precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353–83.
Neary, D., Snowden, J. S., Gustafson, L., Passant, U., Stuss, D., Black, S., Freedman, M., Kertesz, A., Robert, P. H., Albert, M., Boone, K., Miller, B. L., Cummings, J. and Benson, D. F. (1998). Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: a consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurology, 51, 1546–54.
Nestor, P. J., Fryer, T. D. and Hodges, J. R. (2006). Declarative memory impairments in Alzheimer's disease and semantic dementia. NeuroImage, 30, 1010–20.
Newcombe, N. S. and Huttenlocher, J. (2003). Making space: the development of spatial representation and reasoning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Newell, K. M. (1986). Constraints on the development of coordination. In W. Wade and H. T. A. Whiting (eds.), Motor development in children: aspects of coordination and control (pp. 341–60). Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhof.
Nicholls, A. and Kennedy, J. M. (1992). Drawing development: from similarity of features to direction. Child Development, 63, 227–41.
Nicholls, A. and Kennedy, J. M. (1992). Drawing development: from similarity of features to direction. Child Development, 63, 227–41.
Nicholls, A. and Kennedy, J. M. (1995). Foreshortening in cube drawings by children and adults. Perception, 24, 1443–56.
Nicholls, A. L. and Kennedy, J. M. (1992). Drawing development: from similarity of features to direction. Child Development, 63, 227–41.
Nicholls, A. L. and Kennedy, J. M. (1995). Foreshortening in cube drawings by children and adults. Perception, 24, 1443–56.
Nielsen, M., Collier-Baker, E., Davis, J. and Suddendorf, T. (2005). Imitation recognition in a captive chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Animal Cognition, 8, 31–6.
Nielsen, M., Suddendorf, T. and Slaughter, V. (2006). Mirror self-recognition beyond the face. Child Development, 77, 176–85.
Nihei, Y. (1983). Developmental changes in covert principles for the organization of strokes in drawing and handwriting. Acta Psychologica, 54, 846–9.
Ninio, A. and Bruner, J. S. (1978). The achievement and antecedents of labelling. Journal of Child Language, 5, 5–15.
Ninio, A. and Lieblich, A. (1976). The grammar of action: phrase structure in children's copying. Child Development, 47, 846–9.
Nitschke, M. F., Erdmann, C., Trillenberg, P., Sprenger, A., Kock, N., Sperner, J. and Klein, C. (2006). Functional MRI reveals activation of a subcortical network in a 5-year-old girl with genetically confirmed myoclonus-dystonia. Neuropediatrics, 37, 79–82.
Nolan, E. and Kagan, J. (1980). Recognition of self and self's products in preschool children. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 137, 285–94.
Nolan, E. and Kagan, J. (1980b). Recognition of self and self's products in preschool children. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 137, 285–94.
Nolan, E. and Kagan, J. (1981). Memory for products in pre-school children. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 138, 15–25.
Nolan, E. and Kagan, J. (1981). Memory for products in pre-school children. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 138, 15–25.
Nolan, E., Adams, S. and Kagan, J. (1980). Children's recognition memory for their drawings. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 137, 11–15.
Nolan, E., Adams, S. and Kagan, J. (1980a). Children's recognition memory for their drawings. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 137, 11–15.
Pérez-Rigueiro, J., Elices, M., Plaza, G. R., Real, J. I. and Guinea, G. V. (2006). The influence of anaesthesia on the tensile properties of spider silk. Journal of Experimental Biology, 209, 320–6.
Paivio, A., Yuille, J. C. and Madigan, S. A. (1968). Concreteness, imagery, and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76, 1–25.
Panofsky, E. (1927/1991). Perspective as symbolic form. New York: Zone.
Papousek, H. and Papousek, M. (1974). Mirror image and self-recognition in young human infants: I. A new method of experimental analysis. Developmental Psychobiology, 7, 149–57.
Parisi, M. and Marini, F. (1974). Il fenomeno della trasparenza nello sviluppo del grafismo infantile [The transparency phenomenon in the development of graphic skills]. Research report, Contributi dell’ Istituto di Psicologia. Cagliari (Italy): Università di Cagliari.
Parker, S. T. (1998). A social selection model for the evolution and adapative significance of self-conscious emotions. In M. D. Ferrari and R. J. Sternberg (eds.), Self-awareness: its nature and development (pp. 108–34). New York: Guilford Press.
Parker, S. T. and Milbraith, C. (1994). Contributions of imitation and role-playing games to the construction of self in primates. In S. T. Parker, R. W. Mitchell and M. Boccia (eds.), Self-awareness in animals and humans: developmental perspectives (pp. 108–28). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Parker, S. T., Mitchell, R. W. and Boccia, M. (eds.) (1994). Self-awareness in animals and humans: developmental perspectives. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Pascual-Leone, J. (1970). A mathematical model for the transition rule in Piaget's developmental stages. Acta Psychologica, 63, 301–45.
Pascual-Leone, J. (1989). An organismic process model of Witkin's field dependence–independence. In T. Globerson and T. Zelniker (eds.), Cognitive style and cognitive development (pp. 36–70). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Pascual-Leone, J. (2000). Reflections on working memory: are the two models complementary? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 138–54.
Pascual-Leone, J. and Baillargeon, R. (1994). Developmental measurement of mental attention. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 17, 161–200.
Pascual-Leone, J. and Goodman, D. (1979). Intelligence and experience: a neo-Piagetian approach. Instructional Science, 8, 301–67.
Pascual-Leone, J. and Johnson, J. (2004). Affect, self/motivation, and cognitive development: a dialectical constructivist view. In D. Y. Dai and R. J. Sternberg (eds.), Motivation, emotion, and cognition: integrative perspectives on intellectual functioning and development (pp. 197–235). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Pascual-Leone, J. and Johnson, J. (2005). A dialectical constructivist view of developmental intelligence. In O. Wilhelm and R. Engle (eds.), Handbook of understanding and measuring intelligence (pp. 177–201). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Pascual-Leone, J. and Morra, S. (1991). Horizontality of water level: a neo-Piagetian developmental review. In H. W. Reese (ed.), Advances in child development and behaviour (Vol. 23, pp. 231–76). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Pascual-Leone, J., Johnson, J., Baskind, S., Dworsky, S. and Severtson, E. (2000). Culture-fair assessment and the processes of mental attention. In A. Kozulin and Y. Rand (eds.), Experience of mediated learning (pp. 191–214). New York: Pergamon Press.
Patterson, K., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Jefferies, E., Woollams, A., Jones, R., Hodges, J. R. and Rogers, T. T. (2006). ‘Pre-semantic’ cognition in semantic dementia: six deficits in search of an explanation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 169–83.
Patterson, P. and Cohn, R. H. (1994). Self-recognition and self-awareness in lowland gorillas. In S. T. Parker, R. Mitchell and M. Boccia (eds.), Self-awareness in animals and humans: developmental perspectives (pp. 273–90). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Paukner, A., Anderson, J. R., Borelli, E., Visalberghi, E. and Ferrari, P. F. (2005). Macaques (Macaca nemestrina) recognize when they are being imitated. Biological Letters, 1, 219–22.
Pendergrast, M. (2003). Mirror, mirror: a history of the human love affair with reflection. New York: Basic Books.
Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Perner, J., Aichhorn, M., Kronbichler, M., Staffen, W. and Ladurner, G. (2006). Thinking of mental and other representations: the roles of left and right temporo-parietal junction. Social Neuroscience, 1, 245–58.
Perruchet, P. and Vinter, A. (2002). The self-organizing consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 297–330.
Pew, R. W. (1974). Levels of analysis in motor control. Brain Research, 71, 393–400.
Phillips, W. A., Hobbs, S. B. and Pratt, F. R. (1978). Intellectual realism in children's drawings of cubes. Cognition, 6, 15–33.
Phillips, W. A., Hobbs, S. B. and Pratt, F. R. (1978). Intellectual realism in children's drawings of cubes. Cognition, 6, 15–33.
Phillips, W. A., Hobbs, S. B. and Pratt, F. R. (1978). Intellectual realism in children's drawings of cubes. Cognition, 6, 15–33.
Phillips, W. A., Inall, M. and Lauder, E. (1985). On the discovery, storage and use of graphic descriptions. In N. H. Freeman and M. V. Cox (eds.), Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation (pp. 122–34). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Phillips, W. A., Inall, M. and Lauder, E. (1985). On the discovery, storage and use of graphic descriptions. In N. H. Freeman and M. V. Cox (eds.), Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation (pp. 122–34). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Phillips, W. A., Inall, M. and Lauder, E. (1985). On the discovery, storage, and use of graphic descriptions. In N. H. Freeman and M. V. Cox (eds.), Visual order: the nature of development and representation (pp. 122–34). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Piaget, J. (1929). The child's conception of the world. New York: Harcourt, Brace Jovanovich.
Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books.
Piaget, J. (1969). The mechanisms of perception. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Piaget, J. (1977). The role of action in the development of thinking. In F. W. Overton and J. MacCarthy Gallagher (eds.), Knowledge and Development. Vol. 1, Advances in Research and Theory (pp. 17–42). New York: Plenum Press.
Piaget, J. (1981). Le possible et le nécessaire I. L’évolution des possibles chez l'enfant [The possible and the necessary I: the evolution of thinking the possible in the child]. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.
Piaget, J. (1983). Le possible et le nécessaire Ⅱ. L'evolution du nécessaire chez l'enfant [The possible and the necessary Ⅱ. The evolution of thinking the necessary in the child]. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.
Piaget, J. and Inhelder, B. (1947/1967). La représentation de l'espace chez l'enfant. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France (trans. as The child's conception of space. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul).
Piaget, J. and Inhelder, B. (1956). The child's conception of space. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Piaget, J. and Inhelder, B. (1956). The child's conception of space. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Piaget, J. and Inhelder, B. (1956). The child's conception of space. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Piaget, J., Inhelder, B. and Szeminska, A. (1960). The child's conception of geometry. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Picard, D. and Durand, K. (2005). Are young children's drawings canonically biased? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 90, 48–64.
Picard, D. and Durand, K. (2005). Are young children's drawings canonically biased? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 90, 48–64.
Picard, D. and Vinter, A. (1999). Representational flexibility in children's drawing: effects of age and verbal instructions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 605–22.
Picard, D. and Vinter, A. (1999). Representational flexibility in children's drawings: effects of age and verbal instructions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 605–22.
Picard, D. and Vinter, A. (2005). Development of graphic formulas for the depiction of familiar objects. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29, 418–32.
Picard, D. and Vinter, A. (2005). Development of graphic formulas for the depiction of familiar objects. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29, 418–32.
Picard, D. and Vinter, A. (2006). Decomposing and connecting object representations in 5- to 9-year-old children's drawing behaviour. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 529–45.
Picard, D. and Vinter, A. (2006). Decomposing and connecting object representations in 5- to 9-year-old children's drawing behaviour. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 529–45.
Picard, D. and Vinter, A. (2007). Relationship between procedural rigidity and inter-representational change in children's drawing behaviour. Child Development, 74, 521–41.
Pierroutsakos, S. L. and DeLoache, J. S. (2003). Infants' manual investigation of pictured objects varying in realism. Infancy, 4, 141–56.
Pierroutsakos, S. L., DeLoache, J. S., Ground, M. and Bernard, E. N. (2005). Very young children are insensitive to picture- but not object-orientation. Developmental Science, 8, 326–32.
Plotnik, J. M., de Waal, F. B. M. and Reiss, D. (2006). Self-recognition in an Asian elephant. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103, 17053–7.
Porath, M. and Arlin, P. K. (1997). Developmental approaches to artistic giftedness. Creativity Research Journal, 10, 241–50.
Posada, S. and Colell, M. (2005). Mirror responses in a group of Miopithecus talapoin. Primates, 46, 165–72.
Povinelli, D. J. (1995). The unduplicated self. In P. Rochat (ed.), The self in infancy: theory and research (pp. 161–92). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Povinelli, D. J. and Simon, B. B. (1998). Young children's understanding of briefly versus extremely delayed images of the self: emergence of the autobiographical stance. Developmental Psychology, 34, 188–94.
Povinelli, D. J., Rulf, A. B., Landau, K. R. and Bierschwale, D. T. (1993). Self-recognition in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): distribution, ontogeny, and patterns of emergence. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 107, 347–72.
Pylyshyn, Z. (2003). Seeing and visualizing: it's not what you think. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1993). Crystal quest: a search for the basis of maintenance of practised skills into old age. In A. Baddeley and L. Weiskrantz (eds.), Attention: selection, awareness, and control (pp. 188–230). Oxford: Clarendon.
Rakover, S. S. (2002). Featural vs configurational information in faces: a conceptual and empirical analysis. British Journal of Psychology, 93, 1–30.
Rayor, L. S. and Uetz, G. W. (2000). Age-related sequential web building in the colonial spider Metepeira incrassata (Araneidae): an adaptive spacing strategy. Animal Behaviour, 59, 1251–9.
Reddy, V. (2000). Coyness in early infancy. Developmental Science, 3, 186–92.
Reith, E. (1990). Development of representational awareness and performance in drawing production. Archives de Psychologie, 58, 369–79.
Reith, E. and Dominin, D. (1997). The development of children's ability to attend to the visual projection of objects. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 177–96.
Reuning, H. and Wortley, W. (1973). Psychological studies of the Bushmen. Psychologia Africana Monographs. Supplement No. 7.
Rey, A. (1941). L'examen psychologique dans les cas d'encephalopathie traumatique [The psychological assessment of a case of traumatic encephalopathy]. Archives de Psychologie, 28, 286–340.
Ricci, C. (1887). L'arte dei bambini [The art of children]. Bologna.
Ricci, C. (1894). The art of little children. Translated by L. Maitland. Pedagogical Seminary, 3, 302–7. (Original work published 1887.)
Rice, C., Koinis, D., Sullivan, K., Tager-Flusberg, H. and Winner, E. (1997). When 3-year-olds pass the appearance–reality test. Developmental Psychology, 33, 54–61.
Richardson, J. T. E. (1985). Converging operations and reported mediators in the investigation of mental imagery. British Journal of Psychology, 76, 205–14.
Rimland, B. (1978). Savant capabilities of autistic children and their cognitive implications. In G. Serban (ed.), Cognitive defects in the development of mental illness (pp. 43–65). Oxford: Brunner/Mazel.
Risch, P. (1977). Quantitative analysis of orb web patterns in four species of spiders. Behavioral Genetics, 7, 199–238.
Robert, S. (1986). Ontogeny of mirror behaviour in two species of great apes. American Journal of Primatology, 10, 109–17.
Robertson, I. H., Nico, D. and Hood, B. M. (1997). Believing what you feel: using proprioceptive feedback to reduce unilateral neglect. Neuropsychology, 11, 53–8.
Robinson, E. J., Nye, R. and Thomas, G. V. (1994). Children's conceptions of the relationship between pictures and their referents. Cognitive Development, 9, 165–91.
Robinson, J. A., Connell, S., McKenzie, B. E. and Day, R. H. (1990). Do infants use their own images to locate objects reflected in a mirror? Child Devopment, 61, 1558–68.
Rochat, P. and Morgan, R. (1995). Spatial determinants in the perception of self-produced leg movements in 3- to 5-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 31, 626–36.
Rogers, L. J. (2000). Evolution of side bias: motor versus sensory lateralization. In M. K. Mandal, M. B. Bulman-Fleming and G. Tiwari (eds.), Side bias: a neuropsychological perspective (pp. 3–40). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Rogers, T. T., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Garrard, P., Bozeat, S., McClelland, J. L., Hodges, J. R. and Patterson, K. (2004a). The structure and deterioration of semantic memory: a neuropsychological and computational investigation. Psychological Review, 111, 205–35.
Rogers, T. T., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Hodges, J. R. and Patterson, K. (2004b). Natural selection: the impact of semantic impairment on lexical and object decision. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 21, 331–52.
Ropar, D. and Mitchell, P. (2002). Shape constancy in autism: the role of prior knowledge and perspective cues. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 647–53.
Rose, T. L. and Yesavage, J. A. (1983). Differential effects of a list-learning mnemonic in three age-groups. Gerontology, 29, 293–8.
Rosenbaum, D. A. (2005). The Cinderella of psychology: the neglect of motor control in the science of mental life and behaviour. American Psychologist, 60, 308–17.
Rosenbloom, L. and Horton, M. E. (1971). The maturation of fine prehension in young children. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 13, 3–8.
Rosengren, K. S. and Braswell, G. S. (2001). Variability in children's reasoning. In H. W. Reese and R. Kail (eds.), Advances in child development and behaviour (pp. 1–40). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Rosengren, K. S. and Braswell, G. S. (2003). Constraints and the development of children's drawing and writing skills. In G. J. P. Savelsbergh, K. Davids, J. Van der Kamp and S. Bennett (eds.), Development of movement coordination in children: applications in the fields of ergonomics, health sciences, and sport (pp. 56–74). New York: Routledge.
Rosengren, K. S. and Savelsbergh, G. J. P. (2000). A TASC based approach to perceptual motor development. Paper presented at the Motor Development Research Consortium, Bowling Green, Ohio.
Rosengren, K. S., DeGuzman, D. and Pierroutsakos, S. L. (1995). Why can't Johnny draw a square square? Task constraints on children's grip configurations during drawing. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Indianapolis, Indiana.
Rosengren, K. S., Savelsbergh, G. J. P. and Van der Kamp, J. (2003). Development and learning: a TASC-based perspective on the acquisition of perceptual-motor behaviors. Infant Behaviour and Development, 26, 473–94.
Rothi, L. J., Ochipa, C. and Heilman, K. M. (1991). A cognitive neuropsychological model of limb praxis. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 443–58.
Rouma, G. (1913). Le langage graphique de l'enfant [The graphic language of children]. Paris: Alcan.
Russell, C. L., Bard, K. A. and Adamson, L. B. (1997). Social referencing by young chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 111, 185–93.
Russo Pizzo, L. (1974). La trasparenza nel disegno infantile [Transparency in children's drawing]. Il Pisani: Giornale di Patologia Nervosa e Mentale, 98, 179–226.
Russon, A. (2004). Evolutionary reconstructions of great ape intelligence. In A. Russon and D. Begun (eds.), The evolution of thought: evolutionary origins of great ape intelligence (pp. 1–14). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Russon, A. and Bard, K. A. (1996). Exploring the minds of the great apes: issues and controversies. In A. Russon, K. Bard and S. T. Parker (eds.), Reaching into thought: the minds of the great apes (pp. 1–20). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Ruth, J.-E. and Birren, J. E. (1985). Creativity in adulthood and old age: relations to intelligence, sex and mode of testing. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 8, 99–109.
Saida, Y. and Miyashita, M. (1979). Development of fine motor skill in children: manipulation of a pencil in young children ages 2 to 6 years old. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 5, 104–13.
Salthouse, T. A. (1982). Adult cognition: an experimental psychology of human aging. New York: Springer.
Salthouse, T. A. (1984). Effects of age and skill in typing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 11, 345–71.
Salthouse, T. A. (1991). Expertise as the circumvention of human processing limitations. In K. A. Ericsson and J. Smith (eds.), Towards a general theory of expertise: prospects and limits (pp. 286–300). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Sasson, R., Nimmo-Smith, I. and Wing, A. M. (1986). An analysis of children's penholds. In H. S. R. Kao, G. P. Van Galen and R. Hoosian (eds.), Graphonomics: contemporary research in handwriting (pp. 93–106). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Scheirs, J. G. M. (1990). Relationships between the direction of movements and handedness in children. Neuropsychologia, 28, 742–8.
Schier, F. (1986). Deeper into pictures. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Schmuckler, M. A. (1996). Visual-proprioceptive intermodal perception in infancy. Infant Behaviour and Development, 19, 221–32.
Schulman, A. H. and Kaplowitz, C. (1977). Mirror-image response during the first two years of life. Developmental Psychobiology, 10, 133–42.
Schwartz, D. L. (1995). Reasoning about the referent of a picture versus reasoning about the picture as the referent: an effect of visual realism. Memory and Cognition, 23, 709–22.
Schyns, P. G. and Rodet, L. (1997). Categorization creates functional features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 681–96.
Searle, J. R. (1983). Intentionality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Selfe, L. (1977). Nadia: a case of extraordinary drawing ability in an autistic child. London: Academic Press.
Selfe, L. (1983). Normal and anomalous representational drawing ability in children. London: Academic Press.
Selfe, L. (1985). Anomalous drawing development. In N. H. Freeman and M. V. Cox (eds.), Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation (pp. 135–54). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Serpell, R. (1979). How specific are perceptual skills – study of pattern reproduction. British Journal of Psychology, 70, 365–80.
Servos, P., Goodale, M. A. and Humphrey, G. K. (1993). The drawing of objects by a visual form agnosic: contribution of surface properties and memorial representations. Neuropsychologia, 31, 251–9.
Servos, P., Goodale, M. A. and Humphrey, G. K. (1993). The drawing of objects by a visual form agnostic: contribution of surface properties and memorial representations. Neuropsychologia, 31, 251–9.
Sheppard, E., Ropar, D. and Mitchell, P. (2005). The impact of meaning and dimensionality on the accuracy of children's copying. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 365–81.
Sheppard, E., Ropar, D. and Mitchell, P. (2005). The impact of meaning and dimensionality on the accuracy of children's copying. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 365–81.
Sheppard, E., Ropar, D. and Mitchell, P. (2005). The impact of meaning and dimensionality on the accuracy of children's copying. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 365–81.
Sheppard, E., Ropar, D. and Mitchell, P. (2007). The impact of meaning and dimensionality on copying accuracy in individuals with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 37: 1913–24.
Sheppard, E., Ropar, D. and Mitchell, P. (in prep.). Autism and dimensionality: differences between drawing and copying tasks.
Sherick, I., Greenman, G. and Legg, C. (1976). Some comments on the significance and development of midline behaviour during infancy. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 6, 170–83.
Siegler, R. S. (1996). Emerging minds: the process of change in children's thinking. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Silk, A. M. J. and Thomas, G. V. (1986). Development and differentiation in children's figure drawings. British Journal of Psychology, 77, 399–410.
Silk, A. M. J. and Thomas, G. V. (1988). The development of size scaling in children's figure drawings. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 285–99.
Silvia, P. J. and Duval, T. S. (2001). Objective self-awareness theory: recent progress and enduring problems. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 230–41.
Simner, M. L. (1981). The grammar of action and children's printing. Developmental Psychology, 20, 136–42.
Singer, T., Lindenberger, U. and Baltes, P. B. (2003). Plasticity of memory for new learning in very old age: a story of major loss? Psychology and Aging, 18, 306–17.
Slater, A. M., Rose, D. and Morison, V. (1984). Newborn infants' perception of similarities and differences between two and three dimensional stimuli. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2, 287–94.
Smedslund, J. (1969). Psychological diagnostics. Psychological Bulletin, 71, 237–48.
Smith, N. R. (1979). How a picture means. New Directions for Child Development, 3, 59–72.
Snodgrass, J. G. and Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 174–215.
Snyder, A. W. and Thomas, M. (1997). Autistic artists give clues to cognition. Perception, 26, 93–6.
Solso, R. L. (2001). Brain activities in a skilled versus a novice artist: an fMRI study. Leonardo, 34, 31–4.
Spencer, B. (1904). The northern tribes of central Australia. London: Macmillan.
Spencer, J. P. and Hund, A. M. (2003). Developmental continuity in the processes that underlie spatial recall. Cognitive Psychology, 47, 432–80.
Spensley, F. and Taylor, J. (1999). The development of cognitive flexibility: evidence from children's drawings. Human Development, 42, 300–24.
Spensley, F. and Taylor, J. (1999). The development of cognitive flexibility: evidence from children's drawings. Human Development, 42, 300–24.
Stacey, J. T. and Ross, B. M. (1975). Scheme and schema in children's memory of their own drawings. Developmental Psychology, 11, 37–41.
Stacey, J. T. and Ross, B. M. (1975). Scheme and schema in children's memory of their own drawings. Developmental Psychology, 11, 37–41.
Stephan, M. (1990). A transformational theory of aesthetics. London: Routledge.
Stigsdotter Neely, A. and Bäckman, L. (1993). Long-term maintenance of gains from memory training in older adults: two 31/2-year follow-up studies. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 48, P233–P237.
Stiles, J. (1995). The early use and development of graphic formulas: two case study reports of graphic formula production by 2- to 3-year-old children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 18, 127–49.
Stiles, J. (1995). The early use and development of graphic formulas: two case study reports of graphic formula production by 2- to 3-year-old children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 18, 127–49.
Suarez, S. D. and Gallup, G. G., Jr (1981). Self-recognition in chimpanzees and orangutans, but not gorillas. Journal of Human Evolution, 10, 175–88.
Suddendorf, T. (2003). Early representational insight: twenty-four-month-olds can use a photo to find an object in the world. Child Development, 74, 896–904.
Suddendorf, T. and Whiten, A. (2001). Mental evolution and development: evidence for secondary representation in children, great apes, and other animals. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 629–50.
Sutton, P. J. and Rose, D. H. (1998). The role of strategic visual attention in children's drawing development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 68, 87–107.
Sutton, P. J. and Rose, D. H. (1998). The role of strategic visual attention in children's drawing development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 68, 87–107.
Swartz, K. B. (1997). What is mirror self-recognition in nonhuman primates, and what is it not? In J. G. Snodgrass and R. L. Thompson (eds.), The self across psychology: self-recognition, self awareness, and the self concept (pp. 65–71). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.
Swindler, M. H. (1931). Ancient painting. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Tada, W. L. and Stiles, J. (1996). Developmental change in children's analysis of spatial patterns. Developmental Psychology, 3, 951–70.
Tada, W. L. and Stiles, J. (1996). Developmental change in children's analysis of spatial patterns. Developmental Psychology, 32, 951–70.
Tallandini, M. A. and Morassi, L. (2002). Modalità di rappresentazione proiettiva nel disegno infantile [Types of projective representation in children's drawing]. In G. Di Stefano and R. Vianello (eds.), Psicologia dello sviluppo e problemi educativi [Developmental psychology and educational issues]. Florence: Giunti.
Tallandini, M. A. and Morassi, L. (2005). The relation between spatial and symbolic components in children's drawing. Paper presented at the 12th European Conference on Developmental Psychology, 24–28 August.
Tallandini, M. A. and Valentini, P. (1990). Lo sviluppo del disegno infantile: teorie studiali [The development of drawing in children: theoretical studies]. Età Evolutiva, 37, 92–105.
Tallandini, M. A. and Valentini, P. (1991). Strategie rappresentative nel disegno infantile di un oggetto sociale. [Representational strategies in the drawing of a social object in children]. In G. Di Stefano and M.A Tallandini (eds.), Meccanismi e processi di sviluppo: l'interpretazione postpiagetiana [Mechanisms and processes of development: a post-Piagetian perspective]. Milan: Cortina.
Tallandini, M. A. and Varesano, E. (2003). Il processo produttivo nella rappresentazione grafica [The production processes in graphic representations]. Età Evolutiva, 76, 64–71.
Tallandini, M. A., Morassi, L. and Gerbino, W. (1999). The development of representation of tridimensional objects and the relation between spatial and symbolic code. Biennal Meeting of Society for Research in Child Development (SRCD), Albuquerque, New Mexico.
Tessari, A. and Rumiati, R. I. (2004). The strategic control of multiple routes in imitation to actions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 1107–16.
Thelen, E. and Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Thelen, E., Corbetta, D., Kamm, K., Spencer, J. P., Schneider, K. and Zernicke, R. F. (1993). The transition to reaching: mapping intention and intrinsic dynamics. Child Development, 64, 1058–98.
Thomas G. V. and Silk, A. M. J. (1989). An introduction to the psychology of children's drawings. New York: Harvester Wheatsheaf.
Thomas, G. V. (1995). The role of drawing strategies and skills. In C. Lange-Küttner and G. V. Thomas (eds.), Drawing and looking (pp. 107–22). London: Pearson/Prentice Hall (distributed by www.amazon.co.uk).
Thomas, G. V. and Silk, A. M. J. (1990). An introduction to the psychology of children's drawings. New York: New York University Press.
Thomas, G. V., Jolley, R. P., Robinson, E. J. and Champion, H. (1999). Realist errors in children's responses to pictures and words as representations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74, 1–20.
Thomassen, A. J. W. M. (1992). Interaction of cognitive and biomechanical factors in the organization of graphic movements. In G. E. Stelmach and J. Requin (eds.), Tutorials in motor behaviour (Vol. 2, pp. 249–61). Amsterdam: North-Holland/Elsevier.
Thomassen, A. J. W. M. and Teulings, H. H. M. (1979). The development of directional preference in writing movements. Visible Language, 13, 299–313.
Thomassen, A. J. W. M. and Teulings, H. H. M. (1983). The development of handwriting. In M. Martlew (ed.), The psychology of written language (pp. 179–213). New York: Wiley.
Thomassen, A. J. W. M. and Van Galen, G. P. (1992). Handwriting as a motor task. In J. J. Summers (ed.), Approaches to the study of motor control and learning (pp. 113–44). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Thomassen, A. J. W. M., Meulenbroek, R. G. J. and Hoofs, M. P. E. (1992). Economy and anticipation in graphic stroke sequences. Human Movement Science, 11, 71–82.
Thomassen, A. J. W. M., Tibosch, H. J. C. M. and Maarse, F. J. (1989). The effect of context on stroke direction and stroke order in handwriting. In R. Plamondon, C. Y. Suen and M. L. Simner (eds.), Computer recognition and human production of handwriting (pp. 213–30). Singapore: World Scientific.
Thompson, L. A. and Kliegl, R. (1991). Adult age effects of plausibility on memory: the role of time constraints during encoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 542–55.
Thouless, R. H. (1931). Phenomenal regression to the real object. Ⅱ. British Journal of Psychology, 22, 1–30.
Tomasello, M., Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. and Kruger, A. C. (1993). Imitative learning of actions on objects by children, chimpanzees, and enculturated chimpanzees. Child Development, 64, 1688–1705.
Toomela, A. (1999). Drawing development: stages in the representation of a cube and a cylinder. Child Development, 70, 1141–50.
Toomela, A. (2002). Drawing as a verbally mediated activity: a study of relationships between verbal, motor, and visuo-spatial skills and drawing in children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 26, 234–47.
Toomela, A. (2002). Drawing as a verbally mediated activity: a study of relationships between verbal, motor, and visuospatial skills and drawing in children. International Journal of Behavioural Development, 26, 234–47.
Torrance, E. P. (1966a). Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking: directions manual and sorting guide. Princeton, NJ: Personnel Press.
Torrance, E. P. (1966b). Technical-norms manual for the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking: research edition. Princeton, NJ: Personnel Press.
Towse, J. N. and Hitch, G. J. (1996). Performance demands in the selection of objects for counting. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 61, 67–79.
Treat, N. J. and Reese, H. W. (1976). Age, pacing, and imagery in paired-associative learning. Developmental Psychology, 12, 119–24.
Treiman, R., Goswami, U. and Bruck, M. (1990). Not all nonwords are alike: implication for reading development and theory. Memory and Cognition, 18, 559–67.
Treisman, A. (2006). How the deployment of attention determines what we see. Visual Cognition, 14, 411–43.
Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy. In M. Bullowa (ed.), Before speech: the beginning of interpersonal communication (pp. 121–347). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Trevarthen, C. and Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity: confidence, confiding, and acts of meaning in the first year of life. In A. Lock (ed.), Action, gesture, symbol: the emergence of language (pp. 183–229). New York: Academic Press.
Troseth, G. L. and DeLoache, J. S. (1998). The medium can obscure the message: young children's understanding of video. Child Development, 69, 950–65.
Ungerleider, L. G. and Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems. In D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale and R. J. W. Mansfield (eds.), Analysis of visual behaviour (pp. 549–86). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Uttal, D. H., Schreiber, J. C. and DeLoache, J. S. (1995). Waiting to use a symbol: the effects of delay on children's use of models. Child Development, 66, 1875–89.
Van Roon, D., Van der Kamp, J. and Steenbergen, B. (2003). Constraints in children's learning to use spoons. In G. Savelsbergh, K. Davids, J. Van der Kamp and S. J. Bennett (eds.), Development of movement co-ordination in children: applications in the fields of ergonomics, health sciences and sport (pp. 75–93). London: Routledge.
Van Sommers, P. (1983). The conservatism of children's drawing strategies: at what level does stability persist? In D. Rogers and J. A. Sloboda (eds.), The acquisition of symbolic skills (pp. 65–70). New York: Plenum Press.
Van Sommers, P. (1984). Drawing and cognition: descriptive and experimental studies of graphic production processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Van Sommers, P. (1984). Drawing and cognition: descriptive and experimental studies of graphic production processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Van Sommers, P. (1984). Drawing and cognition: descriptive and experimental studies of graphic production processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Van Sommers, P. (1984). Drawing and cognition: descriptive and experimental studies of graphic production processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Van Sommers, P. (1984). Drawing and cognition: descriptive and experimental studies of graphic production processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Van Sommers, P. (1984). Drawing and cognition: descriptive and experimental studies of graphic production processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Van Sommers, P. (1984). Drawing and cognition: descriptive and experimental studies of graphic production processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Van Sommers, P. (1984). Drawings and cognition: descriptive and experimental studies of graphic production processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Van Sommers, P. (1989). A system for drawing and drawing-related neuropsychology. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 6, 117–64.
Vasta, R. and Liben, L. S. (1996). The water-level task: an intriguing puzzle. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 5, 171–7.
Venner, S., Bel-Venner, M. C., Pasquet, A. and Leborgne, R. (2003). Body-mass dependent cost of web-building behaviour in an orb weaving spider, Zygiella x-notata. Naturwissenschaften, 90, 269–72.
Verhaeghen, P. and Marcoen, A. (1996). On the mechanisms of plasticity in young and older adults after instruction in the Method of Loci: evidence for an amplification model. Psychology and Aging, 11, 164–78.
Viggiano, M. P., Righi, S. and Galli, G. (2006). Category-specific visual recognition as affected by aging and expertise. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 42, 329–38.
Vinter, A. (1990). Manual imitations and reaching behaviors: an illustration of action control in infancy. In C. Bard, M. Fleury and L. Hay (eds.), Development of eye–hand coordination across the life span (pp. 157–87). Columbia, SC: University of South Carolina Press.
Vinter, A. (1994). Hierarchy among graphic production rules: a developmental approach. In C. Faure, P. Keuss, G. Lorette and A. Vinter (eds.), Advances in handwriting and drawing: a multidisciplinary approach (pp. 275–88). Paris: Europia.
Vinter, A. (1999). How meaning modifies drawing behaviour in children. Child Development, 70, 33–49.
Vinter, A. (1999). How meaning modifies drawing behaviour in children. Child Development, 70, 33–49.
Vinter, A. (1999). How meaning modifies drawing behaviour in children. Child Development, 70, 33–49.
Vinter, A. and Detable, C. (2003). Implicit learning in children and adolescents with mental retardation. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 108, 94–107.
Vinter, A. and Marot, V. (2007). The development of context sensitivity in children's graphic copying strategies. Developmental Psychology, 43, 94–110.
Vinter, A. and Meulenbroek, R. (1993). The role of manual dominance and visual feedback in circular drawing movements. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 25, 11–37.
Vinter, A. and Perruchet, P. (1999). Isolating unconscious influences: the neutral parameter procedure. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52a, 857–75.
Vinter, A. and Perruchet, P. (2000). Implicit learning in children is not related to age: evidence from drawing behaviour. Child Development, 71, 1223–40.
Vinter, A. and Perruchet, P. (2000). Implicit learning in children is not related to age: evidence from drawing behaviour. Child Development, 71, 1223–40.
Vinter, A. and Perruchet, P. (2002). Implicit motor learning through observational training in adults and children. Memory and Cognition, 30, 256–61.
Vinter, A. and Picard, D. (1996). Drawing behaviour in children reflects internal representational changes. In M. Simner, G. Leedham and A. Thomassen (eds.), Handwriting and drawing research (pp. 171–85). Amsterdam: IOS Press.
Vollrath, F. (2005). Spiders’ webs. Current Biology, 15, R364–R365.
Vollrath, F., Downes, M. and Krackow, S. (1997). Design variability in web geometry of an orb-weaving spider. Physiology and Behaviour, 62, 735–43.
Walsh, D. A. and Hershey, D. A. (1993). Mental models and the maintenance of complex problem solving skills in old age. In J. Cerella, J. Rybash, W. Hoyer and M. L. Commons (eds.), Adult information processing: limits on loss (pp. 553–84). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Wechsler, D. (1964). Der Hamburger-Wechsler-Intelligenztest für Erwachsene (HAWIE). Berne: Huber.
Werner, H. and Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation: an organismic developmental approach to language and the expression of thought. New York: Wiley.
Westergaard, G. C. and Hyatt, C. W. (1994). The responses of bonobos (Pan paniscus) to their mirror images: evidence of self-recognition. Human Evolution, 9, 273–9.
White, R. and Johnson, B. (1930). Children's choices in modern art. Child Development, 1, 347–9.
Willats, J. (1977). How children learn to draw realistic pictures. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 367–82.
Willats, J. (1977). How children learn to draw realistic pictures. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 367–82.
Willats, J. (1977). How children learn to draw realistic pictures. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 367–82.
Willats, J. (1981). What do the marks in the picture stand for? The child's acquisition of systems on transformation and denotation. Review of Research in Visual Arts Education, 13, 18–33.
Willats, J. (1985). Drawing systems revisited: the role of denotation systems in children's figure drawing. In N. H. Freeman and M. V. Cox (eds.), Visual order: the nature of development and representation (pp. 78–100). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Willats, J. (1985). Drawing systems revisited: the role of denotation systems in children's figure drawings. In N. H. Freeman and M. V. Cox (eds.), Visual order (pp. 78–100). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Willats, J. (1985). Drawing systems revisited: the role of denotation systems in children's figure drawings. In N. H. Freeman and M. V. Cox (eds.), Visual order (pp. 78–100). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Willats, J. (1985). Drawing systems revisited: the role of denotation systems in children's figure drawings. In N. H. Freeman and M. V. Cox (eds.), Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation (pp. 78–100). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Willats, J. (1987). Marr and pictures: an information-processing account of children's drawings. Archives de Psychologie, 55, 105–25.
Willats, J. (1995). An information-processing approach to drawing development. In C. Lange-Küttner and G. V. Thomas (eds.), Drawing and looking (pp. 27–43). London: Pearson/Prentice Hall (distributed by www.amazon.co.uk).
Willats, J. (1995). An information-processing approach to drawing development. In C. Lange-Küttner and G. V. Thomas (eds.), Drawing and looking: theoretical approaches to pictorial representation in children (pp. 27–43). London: Pearson/Prentice Hall (distributed by www.amazon.co.uk).
Willats, J. (1997). Art and representation. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Willats, J. (1997). Art and representation. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Willats, J. (1997). Art and representation. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Willats, J. (1997). Art and representation: new principles in the analysis of pictures. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Willats, J. (2003). Optical laws or symbolic rules? The dual nature of pictorial systems. In H. Hecht and R. Schwartz (eds.), Looking into pictures: an interdisciplinary approach to pictorial space (pp. 125–43). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Willats, J. (2003). Optical laws or symbolic rules? The dual nature of pictorial systems. In H. Hecht, R. Schwartz and M. Atherton (eds.), Looking into pictures: an interdisciplinary approach to pictorial space (pp. 125–43). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Willats, J. (2005). Making sense of children's drawing. London: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Willats, J. (2005). Making sense of children's drawings. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Willats, J. (2005). Making sense of children's drawings. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Wilson, M. and Wilson, B. (1982). The case of disappearing two-eyed profile: or how little children influence drawings of little children. Research in Visual Arts Education, 15, 19–32.
Wilson, M. and Wilson, B. (1982). The case of the disappearing of the two-eyed profile or how little children influence the drawings of little children. Review of Research in Visual Arts Education, 15, 19–32.
Wong, T. H. and Kao, H. S. R. (1991). The development of drawing principles in Chinese. In J. Wann, A. M. Wing and N. S⊘vik (eds.), Development of graphic skills (pp. 93–112). New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Wulf, G. and Schmidt, R. A. (1997). Variability of practice and implicit motor learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 23, 987–1006.
Young, A. W. and Deręgowski, J. B. (1981). Learning to see the impossible. Perception, 10, 91–105.
Zaitchik, D. (1990). When representations conflict with reality: the preschooler's problem with false beliefs and false photographs. Cognition, 35, 41–68.
Zelazo, P. D., Frye, D. and Rapus, T. (1996). An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them. Cognitive Development, 11, 37–63.
Zhi, Z., Thomas, G. V. and Robinson, E. J. (1997). Constraints on representational change: drawing a man with two heads. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 275–90.
Zhi, Z., Thomas, G. V. and Robinson, E. J. (1997). Constraints on representational change: drawing a man with two heads. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 275–90.
Zhi, Z., Thomas, G. V. and Robinson, E. J. (1997). Constraints on representational change: drawing a man with two heads. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 275–90.
Zhi, Z., Thomas, G. V. and Robinson, E. J. (1997). Constraints on representational changes: drawing a man with two heads. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 275–90.
Ziviani, J. (1982). Children's prehension while writing – a pilot investigation. British Journal of Occupational Therapy, 45, 306–7.
Ziviani, J. (1983). Qualitative changes in dynamic tripod grip between seven and 14 years of age. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 25, 535–9.
Ziviani, J. and Elkins, J. (1986). Effect of pencil grip on handwriting speed and legibility. Educational Review, 38, 247–57.